The development of selective stopping: Qualitative and quantitative changes from childhood to early adulthood
- PMID: 34873804
- DOI: 10.1111/desc.13210
The development of selective stopping: Qualitative and quantitative changes from childhood to early adulthood
Abstract
Although progress has been made in elucidating the behavioral and neural development of global stopping across the lifespan, little is known about the development of selective stopping. This more complex form of inhibitory control is required in real-world situations where ongoing responses must be inhibited to certain stimuli but not others, and can be assessed in laboratory settings using a stimulus selective stopping task. Here we used this task to investigate the qualitative and quantitative developmental changes in selective stopping in a large-scale cross-sectional study with three different age groups (children, preadolescents, and young adults). We found that the ability to stop a response selectively to some stimuli (i.e., use a selective strategy) rather than non-selectively to all presented stimuli (i.e., use a global, non-selective strategy) is fully mature by early preadolescence, and remains stable afterwards at least until young adulthood. By contrast, the efficiency or speed of stopping (indexed by a shorter stop-signal reaction time or SSRT) continues to mature throughout adolescence until young adulthood, both for global and selective implementations of stopping. We also provide some preliminary findings regarding which other task variables beyond the strategy and SSRT predicted age group status. Premature responding (an index of "waiting impulsivity") and post-ignore slowing (an index of cognitive control) were among the most relevant predictors in discriminating between developmental age groups. Although present results need to be confirmed and extended in longitudinal studies, they provide new insights into the development of a relevant form of inhibitory control.
Keywords: SSRT; cognitive control; development; response inhibition; selective stopping; strategies.
© 2021 The Authors. Developmental Science published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Similar articles
-
Selective Inhibitory Control in Middle Childhood.Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021 Jun 10;18(12):6300. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18126300. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021. PMID: 34200757 Free PMC article.
-
The effects of discrimination on the adoption of different strategies in selective stopping.Psychon Bull Rev. 2021 Feb;28(1):209-218. doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01797-6. Psychon Bull Rev. 2021. PMID: 32815113
-
Neural and behavioral correlates of selective stopping: Evidence for a different strategy adoption.Neuroimage. 2016 Oct 1;139:279-293. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.043. Epub 2016 Jun 26. Neuroimage. 2016. PMID: 27355436
-
Stopping Interference in Response Inhibition: Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Selective Stopping.J Neurosci. 2022 Jan 12;42(2):156-165. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0668-21.2021. J Neurosci. 2022. PMID: 35022327 Free PMC article. Review.
-
Attention deficit and impulsivity: selecting, shifting, and stopping.Int J Psychophysiol. 2005 Oct;58(1):59-70. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.009. Int J Psychophysiol. 2005. PMID: 15950304 Review.
Cited by
-
Comparing anticipatory and stop-signal response inhibition with a novel, open-source selective stopping toolbox.Exp Brain Res. 2023 Feb;241(2):601-613. doi: 10.1007/s00221-022-06539-9. Epub 2023 Jan 13. Exp Brain Res. 2023. PMID: 36635589 Free PMC article.
-
Inhibition of return emerges with non-predictive spatial cueing of the stop-signal.Front Hum Neurosci. 2025 Jul 2;19:1567597. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2025.1567597. eCollection 2025. Front Hum Neurosci. 2025. PMID: 40672741 Free PMC article.
References
REFERENCES
-
- Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), e55-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024
-
- Bari, A., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology, 108, 44-79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005
-
- Bartholdy, S., Dalton, B., O'Daly, O. G., Campbell, I. C., & Schmidt, U. (2016). A systematic review of the relationship between eating, weight and inhibitory control using the stop signal task. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 35-62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.010
-
- Bedard, A. C., Nichols, S., Barbosa, J. A., Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (2002). The development of selective inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 93-111. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2101_5
-
- Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources