Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Jan 7:12:790438.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.790438. eCollection 2021.

Computer-Based Assessment: Dual-Task Outperforms Large-Screen Cancellation Task in Detecting Contralesional Omissions

Affiliations

Computer-Based Assessment: Dual-Task Outperforms Large-Screen Cancellation Task in Detecting Contralesional Omissions

Sanna Villarreal et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Objective: Traditionally, asymmetric spatial processing (i.e., hemispatial neglect) has been assessed with paper-and-pencil tasks, but growing evidence indicates that computer-based methods are a more sensitive assessment modality. It is not known, however, whether simply converting well-established paper-and-pencil methods into a digital format is the best option. The aim of the present study was to compare sensitivity in detecting contralesional omissions of two different computer-based methods: a "digitally converted" cancellation task was compared with a computer-based Visual and Auditory dual-tasking approach, which has already proved to be very sensitive. Methods: Participants included 40 patients with chronic unilateral stroke in either the right hemisphere (RH patients, N = 20) or the left hemisphere (LH patients, N = 20) and 20 age-matched healthy controls. The cancellation task was implemented on a very large format (173 cm × 277 cm) or in a smaller (A4) paper-and-pencil version. The computer-based dual-tasks were implemented on a 15'' monitor and required the detection of unilateral and bilateral briefly presented lateralized targets. Results: Neither version of the cancellation task was able to show spatial bias in RH patients. In contrast, in the Visual dual-task RH patients missed significantly more left-sided targets than controls in both unilateral and bilateral trials. They also missed significantly more left-sided than right-sided targets only in the bilateral trials of the Auditory dual-task. Conclusion: The dual-task setting outperforms the cancellation task approach even when the latter is implemented on a (large) screen. Attentionally demanding methods are useful for revealing mild forms of contralesional visuospatial deficits.

Keywords: computer-based methods; dual-task; extinction; hemispatial neglect; neuropsychological evaluation; neuropsychology; paper-and-pencil tasks; stroke.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
The large-screen Twinkle Task: demonstrations of empty task sheet (left), and correctly selected targets (right).
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
The large-screen Twinkle Task: visualization of the scanning pattern and the selected targets in a representative patient.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
A representative trial of the Visual dual-task (image not in scale). A left target (alternative positions possible: right or bilateral) is briefly presented (50 ms) concurrently with a central letter and an auditory digit. Correct responses for the Visual dual-task would have been “a” (central letter) and “left” (target position), while for the Auditory dual-task they would have been “four, six” (count in steps of two from the auditorily presented “two”) and “left.” (Adapted from Bonato, 2015).
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
The Visual dual-task: average percentages of missed targets within each group in unilateral (top panel) and bilateral (bottom panel) trials. RH patients missed significantly more left-sided targets than controls in both the unilateral and bilateral trials. Asterisks represent effect size (***large effect; **medium effect).
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
The Auditory dual-task: average percentages of missed targets within each group in unilateral (top panel) and bilateral (bottom panel) trials. RH patients missed significantly more left-sided than right-sided targets in bilateral trials. Asterisks represent effect size (***large effect).

References

    1. Aglioti S., Smania N., Barbieri C., Corbetta M. (1997). Influence of stimulus salience and attentional demands on visual search patterns in hemispatial neglect. Brain Cogn. 34 388–403. 10.1006/brcg.1997.0915 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Albert M. L. (1973). A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology 23 658–664. 10.1212/wnl.23.6.658 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Andres M., Geers L., Marnette S., Coyette F., Bonato M., Priftis K., et al. (2019). Increased cognitive load reveals unilateral neglect and altitudinal extinction in chronic stroke. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 21 1–10. 10.1017/S1355617719000249 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Azouvi P., Samuel C., Louis-Dreyfus A., Bernati T., Bartolomeo P., Beis J. M., et al. (2002). Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 73 160–166. 10.1136/jnnp.73.2.160 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bartolomeo P., Chokron S. (1999). Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention? Neurology 53 2023–2027. 10.1212/wnl.53.9.2023 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources