Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Feb 3;12(1):1863.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05495-2.

Adapting to the pandemic: longitudinal effects of social restrictions on time perception and boredom during the Covid-19 pandemic in Germany

Affiliations

Adapting to the pandemic: longitudinal effects of social restrictions on time perception and boredom during the Covid-19 pandemic in Germany

Marlene Wessels et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

With the Covid-19 pandemic, many governments introduced nationwide lockdowns that disrupted people's daily routines and promoted social isolation. We applied a longitudinal online survey to investigate the mid-term effects of the mandated restrictions on the perceived passage of time (PPT) and boredom during and after a strict lockdown in Germany. One week after the beginning of the lockdown in March 2020, respondents reported a slower PPT and increased boredom compared to the pre-pandemic level. However, in the course of the lockdown, PPT accelerated and boredom decreased again until August 2020. Then, in October 2020, when incidence rates sharply rose and new restrictions were introduced, we again observed a slight trend toward a slowing of PPT and an increase of boredom. Our data also show that as the pandemic progressed, respondents adjusted their predictions about the pandemic's duration substantially upward. In sum, our findings suggest that respondents adapted to the pandemic situation and anticipated it as the new "normal". Furthermore, we determined perceived boredom and the general emotional state to be predictive of PPT, while depressive symptoms played a minor role.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean ratings for the perceived passage of time (PPT) and boredom, both before (pre-pandemic) and during the pandemic as a function of measurement points. The main study included measurement points 1–10 (gray background). The follow-up study included measurement points 11 and 12 (white background). Error bars show 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 2
Figure 2
(a) Mean differences between the pandemic and pre-pandemic ratings are depicted for the perceived passage of time (PPT) (blue triangle) and boredom (green square), as a function of measurement point. The dotted horizontal line marks the level of no difference between pandemic and pre-pandemic ratings. Positive values indicate faster pandemic PPT/higher pandemic boredom compared to the pre-pandemic ratings. (b) Mean valence ratings (left y-axis) and mean PHQ scores (right y-axis) during the pandemic are displayed as a function of measurement point. Note that higher PHQ scores (purple diamond) indicate more depressive symptoms. Higher valence ratings (orange circles) indicate a positive emotional state, while lower values represent a negative emotional state. A value of 3 indicates a “neutral” state. The main study included measurement points 1–10 (gray background). The follow-up study included the measurement points 11 and 12 (white background). Error bars show 95% confidence intervals of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean estimated durations of how long respondents expected the pandemic to last as a function of measurement point. The main study included measurement points 1–10 (gray background). The follow-up study included measurement points 11 and 12 (white background). Error bars show 95% confidence intervals.

References

    1. Wang C, et al. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:1729. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051729. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Husky MM, Kovess-Masfety V, Swendsen JD. Stress and anxiety among university students in France during Covid-19 mandatory confinement. Comp. Psychiatry. 2020;102:152191. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152191. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Tso IF, Park S. Alarming levels of psychiatric symptoms and the role of loneliness during the COVID-19 epidemic: A case study of Hong Kong. Psychiatry Res. 2020;293:113423. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113423. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Lee H-S, Dean D, Baxter T, Griffith T, Park S. Deterioration of mental health despite successful control of the COVID-19 pandemic in South Korea. Psychiatry Res. 2021;295:113570. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113570. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Caci B, Miceli S, Scrima F, Cardaci M. Neuroticism and fear of COVID-19: The interplay between boredom fantasy engagement and perceived control over time. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:2578. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574393. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types