Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2022 Feb 4;7(1):11.
doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00350-w.

Cross-cultural differences in visuo-spatial processing and the culture-fairness of visuo-spatial intelligence tests: an integrative review and a model for matrices tasks

Affiliations
Review

Cross-cultural differences in visuo-spatial processing and the culture-fairness of visuo-spatial intelligence tests: an integrative review and a model for matrices tasks

Corentin Gonthier. Cogn Res Princ Implic. .

Abstract

Visuo-spatial reasoning tests, such as Raven's matrices, Cattell's culture-fair test, or various subtests of the Wechsler scales, are frequently used to estimate intelligence scores in the context of inter-racial comparisons. This has led to several high-profile works claiming that certain ethnic groups have lower intelligence than others, presumably due to genetic inferiority. This logic is predicated on the assumption that such visuo-spatial tests, because they are non-verbal, must be culture-fair: that their solution process does not significantly draw on factors that vary from one culture to the next. This assumption of culture-fairness is dubious at best and has been questioned by many authors. In this article, I review the substantial body of psychological and ethnographic literature which has demonstrated that the perception, manipulation and conceptualization of visuo-spatial information differs significantly across cultures, in a way that is relevant to intelligence tests. I then outline a model of how these inter-cultural differences can affect seven major steps of the solution process for Raven's matrices, with a brief discussion of other visuo-spatial reasoning tests. Overall, a number of cultural assumptions appear to be deeply ingrained in all visuo-spatial reasoning tests, to the extent that it disqualifies the view of such tests as intrinsically culture-fair and makes it impossible to draw clear-cut conclusions from average score differences between ethnic groups.

Keywords: Cross-cultural psychology; Culture-fair; Fluid intelligence; Method bias; Raven's matrices; Visuo-spatial reasoning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The author declares no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Example item for a matrix task. Note This example is fairly typical of what can be encountered in a matrix task. It is somewhat more difficult than most items in the versions of Raven's matrices usually employed for cross-cultural comparisons (due to more rules being included at the same time: distribution of three, movement, pairwise progression), and it uses more colors (but less than other versions, such as those of the Wechsler scales)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Example item for a block design test. Note This item is from the original version of Kohs' (1920) block design test. The target pattern, printed on a sheet of paper, has to be reproduced using cubes (whose sides are printed red, white, blue, yellow, red-and-white, and blue-and-yellow). The test is timed. Each design uses between 4 and 16 blocks (9 in the above example). Some designs are presented with the reference frame oriented as a diamond

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abdel-Khalek A, Raven J. Kuwaiti norms for the classic SPM in an international context. In: Raven J, Raven J, editors. Uses and abuses of intelligence: Studies advancing Spearman and Raven's quest for non-arbitrary metrics. Royal Fireworks Press; 2000.
    1. Abecassis M, Sera MD, Yonas A, Schwade J. What’s in a shape? Children represent shape variability differently than adults when naming objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2001;78(3):213–239. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2573. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Abed F. Cultural influences on visual scanning patterns. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1991;22(4):525–534. doi: 10.1177/0022022191224006. - DOI
    1. Agranovich AV. The culture of time in neuropsychological assessment—Exploring the effects of culture-specific time attitudes on timed test performance in Russian and American samples. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2011;17(4):692–701. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000592. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Allport GW, Pettigrew TF. Cultural influence on the perception of movement: The trapezoidal illusion among Zulus. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1957;55(1):104–113. doi: 10.1037/h0049372. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources