Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Feb 14;12(1):2447.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-06396-0.

Using citizen science to test for acoustic niche partitioning in frogs

Affiliations

Using citizen science to test for acoustic niche partitioning in frogs

Slade Allen-Ankins et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

The acoustic niche hypothesis proposes that to avoid interference with breeding signals, vocal species should evolve to partition acoustic space, minimising similarity with co-occurring signals. Tests of the acoustic niche hypothesis are typically conducted using a single assemblage, with mixed outcomes, but if the process is evolutionarily important, a pattern of reduced acoustic competition should emerge, on average, over many communities. Using a continental-scale dataset derived from audio recordings collected by citizen scientists, we show that frogs do partition acoustic space. Differences in calls were predominately caused by differences in spectral, rather than temporal, features. Specifically, the 90% frequency bandwidths of observed frog assemblages overlapped less than expected, and there was greater distance between dominant frequencies than expected. To our knowledge, this study is the first to use null models to test for acoustic niche partitioning over a large geographic scale.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
(a) Spectrogram, oscillogram and power spectrum of sample frog call (Adelotus brevis) showing the various call parameters measured to determine acoustic similarity among calls of different frog species. (b) Map of records used for spectral overlap, PCAAll, PCASpectral and PCATemporal (red points, n = 1641), and dominant frequency distance (red and black points, n = 1854) analyses overlayed on Australia’s 7 ecoregions representing the different broad habitat types from which frog assemblages originate (TrFo = Tropical and Subtropical Moist Broaleaf Forests, TeFo = Temperate Broadleaf and Mixed Forests, TrGr = Tropical and Subtropical Grasslands, Savannas and Shrublands, TeGr = Temperatre Grasslands, Savannas and Shrublands, MoGr = Montane Grasslands and Shrublands, MeFo = Mediterranean Forests, Woodlands and Scrub, DeSh = Deserts and Xeric Shrublands). Figures generated using R version 3.6.1 (https://www.r-project.org/). Annotations on a) added with GIMP version 2.10.6 (https://www.gimp.org/).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Standardised effect sizes of mean pairwise differences between observed and null assemblages for each measure of acoustic similarity. PCAX measures represent the Euclidean distances between calls from the first three principal components of a PCA on either all measured call parameters, spectral call parameters only, or temporal call parameters only. Spectral overlap is the proportion of overlap between species 90% frequency bandwidths. Dominant frequency distance is the difference between species dominant frequencies in log10 Hz. Positive values indicate a trend towards acoustic niche partitioning, while negative values indicate a trend towards acoustic niche aggregation. Note The sign of values for spectral overlap have been reversed so that they are comparable with the other four acoustic similarity measures which are all based on distance. Figure generated using R version 3.6.1 (https://www.r-project.org/).

References

    1. Schwartz, J. J. & Bee, M. A. in Animal communication and noise (ed Henrik Brumm) 91–132 (Springer, 2013).
    1. Wollerman L. Acoustic interference limits call detection in a Neotropical frog Hyla ebraccata. Anim. Behav. 1999;57:529–536. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.1013. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Gerhardt HC, Schwartz JJ. Interspecific interactions in anuran courtship. Amphib. Biol. 1995;2:603–632.
    1. Gröning J, Hochkirch A. Reproductive interference between animal species. Q. Rev. Biol. 2008;83:257–282. doi: 10.1086/590510. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Popp JW, Ficken RW, Reinartz JA. Short-term temporal avoidance of interspecific acoustic interference among forest birds. Auk. 1985;102:744–748. doi: 10.1093/auk/102.4.744. - DOI

Publication types