Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Mar 4:4:844436.
doi: 10.3389/fspor.2022.844436. eCollection 2022.

Improved Walking Through an Aperture in a Virtual Environment Transfers to a Real Environment: Introduction of Enriched Feedback and Gradual Increase in Task Difficulty

Affiliations

Improved Walking Through an Aperture in a Virtual Environment Transfers to a Real Environment: Introduction of Enriched Feedback and Gradual Increase in Task Difficulty

Yuki Suda et al. Front Sports Act Living. .

Abstract

Virtual reality (VR) could be used to set up a training protocol to improve one's collision-avoidance behavior. In our previous study, we developed a VR system for training older individuals to walk through an aperture in a manner that is both safe (i.e., no collision) and efficient (i.e., no exaggerated behavior to ensure collision avoidance). In the present study, we made several modifications to the VR system in terms of enriched feedback (vibratory stimulation for virtual collisions and the addition of positive feedback for successful trials) and gradual increase in task difficulty during training to strengthen the skill transfer. Nineteen older adults (74.4 ± 5.3 years of age) and 21 younger adults (25.1 ± 5.0 years of age) participated. They were randomly assigned to one of two training groups: the intervention group (older: n = 10; younger: n = 10) or the control group (older: n = 11; younger: n = 9). The experiment consisted of pre- and post-training tests in a real environment and training in a VR environment. During training, participants held a horizontal bar while stepping in place as if a VR image on the screen were moving in response to their stepping. Participants in the intervention group tried to pass a narrow aperture without collision while attempting to minimize their body rotation to avoid collision as much as possible. The criterion upon which the collision-avoidance behavior was regarded as successful became incrementally more demanding as participants successfully met the previous criterion. Participants in the control group passed through a very wide aperture, so that collision-avoidance behavior was unnecessary. A comparison between pre- and post-training test performances showed that, for both older and younger adults in the intervention group, the spatial margins became significantly smaller, while the success rate remained unchanged. For those in the control group, neither the spatial margin nor the success rate was improved. These results suggest that the three modifications made for the VR system contributed to improvement of the system and helped participants transfer the behavior learned from the VR environment to real walking.

Keywords: motor learning; obstacle avoidance; older adults; stepping in place; virtual reality.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Experimental procedures of this study.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Real environment for pre- and post-training tests.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Marker setup and equipment: (A) horizontal bar, (B) stereo shutter glasses, (C) knee pad, and (D) vibration stimulator. Yellow markers show reflexive markers.
Figure 4
Figure 4
VR environment for training.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Graphs of the collision rate. Error bars represent the standard deviation among participants. * indicates the significant difference.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Graphs of the mean spatial margin. Error bars represent the standard deviation among participants. * indicates the significant difference.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Graphs of the mean absolute angle of body rotation. Error bars represent the standard deviation among participants. * indicates the significant difference.
Figure 8
Figure 8
Graphs of the relative distance between mid-body and mid-doorway. Error bars represent the standard deviation among participants. * indicates the significant difference.
Figure 9
Figure 9
The mean value of the critical value for older and younger adults. Error bars represent the standard deviation among participants.

References

    1. Abe M., Schambra H., Wassermann E. M., Luckenbaugh D., Schweighofer N., Cohen L. G. (2011). Reward improves long-term retention of a motor memory through induction of offline memory gains. Curr. Biol. 21, 557–562. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.02.030 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Akizuki K., Ohashi Y. (2015). Measurement of functional task difficulty during motor learning: what level of difficulty corresponds to the optimal challenge point? Hum. Mov. Sci. 43, 107–117. 10.1016/j.humov.2015.07.007 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Blakemore S. J., Wolpert D. M., Frith C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 237–242. 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Cavallo V., Dommes A., Dang N.-T., Vienne F. (2019). A street-crossing simulator for studying and training pedestrians. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 61, 217–228. 10.1016/j.trf.2017.04.012 - DOI
    1. Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E., McHugh P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 - DOI - PubMed