Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 May 23;377(1851):20210151.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0151. Epub 2022 Apr 4.

Correlates of individual participation in boundary patrols by male chimpanzees

Affiliations

Correlates of individual participation in boundary patrols by male chimpanzees

Anthony P Massaro et al. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. .

Abstract

Group territory defence poses a collective action problem: individuals can free-ride, benefiting without paying the costs. Individual heterogeneity has been proposed to solve such problems, as individuals high in reproductive success, rank, fighting ability or motivation may benefit from defending territories even if others free-ride. To test this hypothesis, we analysed 30 years of data from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the Kasekela community, Gombe National Park, Tanzania (1978-2007). We examined the extent to which individual participation in patrols varied according to correlates of reproductive success (mating rate, rank, age), fighting ability (hunting), motivation (scores from personality ratings), costs of defecting (the number of adult males in the community) and gregariousness (sighting frequency). By contrast to expectations from collective action theory, males participated in patrols at consistently high rates (mean ± s.d. = 74.5 ± 11.1% of patrols, n = 23 males). The best predictors of patrol participation were sighting frequency, age and hunting participation. Current and former alpha males did not participate at a higher rate than males that never achieved alpha status. These findings suggest that the temptation to free-ride is low, and that a mutualistic mechanism such as group augmentation may better explain individual participation in group territorial behaviour. This article is part of the theme issue 'Intergroup conflict across taxa'.

Keywords: Pan troglodytes; boundary patrols; collection action problems; hunting; intergroup aggression; territorial behaviour.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

We declare we have no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Boundary patrol participation for 24 Kasekela males. Column height indicates the percentage of patrols that each individual participated in while greater than 12 years old. Numbers above the columns indicate the total number of patrols that each individual joined (numerator), and the number of patrols recorded while that individual was alive and ≥12 years old (denominator). All current and former alpha males are labelled with an alpha (α). Here, we include PX, who suffered a scrotal wound at a young age and was effectively castrated. We excluded PX from analyses.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Boundary patrol participation versus sighting frequency. Each point represents the percentage of patrols in which a male participated for a given year. Plotted regression line represents the probability of a male participating in a given patrol relative to annual sighting frequency. The grey area represents the 95% confidence interval for the regression line. (Online version in colour.)
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Boundary patrol participation versus hunting participation. Each point represents the percentage of patrols in which a male participated for a given year. Plotted regression line represents the probability of a male participating in a given patrol relative to annual hunting participation. The grey area represents the 95% confidence interval for the regression line. (Online version in colour.)

References

    1. Mares R, Young AJ, Clutton-Brock TH. 2012. Individual contributions to territory defence in a cooperative breeder: weighing up the benefits and costs. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 3989-3995. (10.1098/RSPB.2012.1071) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bonanni R, Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. 2010. Effect of affiliative and agonistic relationships on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Anim. Behav. 79, 981-991. (10.1016/J.ANBEHAV.2010.02.021) - DOI
    1. Heinsohn R, Packer C. 1995. Complex cooperative strategies in group-territorial African lions. Science 269, 1260-1262. (10.1126/SCIENCE.7652573) - DOI - PubMed
    1. Nunn CL, Deaner RO. 2004. Patterns of participation and free riding in territorial conflicts among ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 57, 50-61. (10.1007/s00265-004-0830-5) - DOI
    1. Crofoot MC, Gilby IC. 2012. Cheating monkeys undermine group strength in enemy territory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 501-505. (10.1073/PNAS.1115937109) - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources