Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Aug;53(8):3220-3229.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-022-05569-0. Epub 2022 Jun 3.

Attention Allocation During Exploration of Visual Arrays in ASD: Results from the ABC-CT Feasibility Study

Affiliations

Attention Allocation During Exploration of Visual Arrays in ASD: Results from the ABC-CT Feasibility Study

Tawny Tsang et al. J Autism Dev Disord. 2023 Aug.

Abstract

Visual exploration paradigms involving object arrays have been used to examine salience of social stimuli such as faces in ASD. Recent work suggests performance on these paradigms may associate with clinical features of ASD. We evaluate metrics from a visual exploration paradigm in 4-to-11-year-old children with ASD (n = 23; 18 males) and typical development (TD; n = 23; 13 males). Presented with arrays containing faces and nonsocial stimuli, children with ASD looked less at (p = 0.002) and showed fewer fixations to (p = 0.022) faces than TD children, and spent less time looking at each object on average (p = 0.004). Attention to the screen and faces correlated positively with social and cognitive skills in the ASD group (ps < .05). This work furthers our understanding of objective measures of visual exploration in ASD and its potential for quantifying features of ASD.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder; Eye-tracking; Visual exploration; Visual processing; Visual search.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Sample stimulus from the visual exploration paradigm. Each circular array had 5 image stimuli featuring one colored photo from the following categories: faces, scrambled faces, mobile phones, birds, and cars. *Note, black bar over eyes of lower right face was not shown to participants.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
A) Percentage of on-screen looking time ASD and TD groups spent looking at face and non-face objects, with on-screen looking time (Valid Looking) percentage of stimulus display time also shown for reference. B) Number of fixations on objects by item type and group (i.e. detail orientation). C) Number of objects viewed per trial out of a maximum of 5 (i.e. exploration). D) Amount of time spent looking at a viewed object on average (i.e., perseveration). *p<.05, **p<.01

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, Va.: American Psychiatric Association.
    1. Ben-Itzchak Esther, and Zachor Ditza A.. 2007. “The Effects of Intellectual Functioning and Autism Severity on Outcome of Early Behavioral Intervention for Children with Autism.” Research in Developmental Disabilities 28(3):287–303. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2006.03.002. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brenner Laurie a, Turner Katherine C., and Müller Ralph-Axel. 2007. “Eye Movement and Visual Search: Are There Elementary Abnormalities in Autism?” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37(7):1289–1309. - PubMed
    1. Chawarska Katarzyna, Macari Suzanne L., and Shic Frederick. 2013. “Decreased Spontaneous Attention to Social Scenes in 6-Month-Old Infants Later Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorders.” Biological Psychiatry 74(3):195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.022. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cheung CHM, Bedford R, Johnson MH, Charman T, and Gliga T. 2016. “Visual Search Performance in Infants Associates with Later ASD Diagnosis.” Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.09.003. - DOI - PMC - PubMed