Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2022 May 20:16:846369.
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.846369. eCollection 2022.

Cognitive Neural Mechanism of Backward Inhibition and Deinhibition: A Review

Affiliations
Review

Cognitive Neural Mechanism of Backward Inhibition and Deinhibition: A Review

Jiwen Chen et al. Front Behav Neurosci. .

Abstract

Task switching is one of the typical paradigms to study cognitive control. When switching back to a recently inhibited task (e.g., "A" in an ABA sequence), the performance is often worse compared to a task without N-2 task repetitions (e.g., CBA). This difference is called the backward inhibitory effect (BI effect), which reflects the process of overcoming residual inhibition from a recently performed task (i.e., deinhibition). The neural mechanism of backward inhibition and deinhibition has received a lot of attention in the past decade. Multiple brain regions, including the frontal lobe, parietal, basal ganglia, and cerebellum, are activated during deinhibition. The event-related potentials (ERP) studies have shown that deinhibition process is reflected in the P1/N1 and P3 components, which might be related to early attention control, context updating, and response selection, respectively. Future research can use a variety of new paradigms to separate the neural mechanisms of BI and deinhibition.

Keywords: backward inhibition; cognitive control; deinhibition; n-2 repetitive costs; task switching.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
The illustration of the sub-process in the ABA (BI) and CBA (base) condition. Inhibiting A(C) means that both the task A and C should be inhibited when performing task B, while task A needs more inhibition than task C. Inhibiting A (in gray color) means the residual inhibition of task A.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Illustration of the disassociation between inhibition and deinhibition in task sequence with three different tasks of increasing difficulty. The letter with the smallest size indicates that a particular task is easiest, while the letter with largest size indicates that it is the most difficulty one.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abrahamse E., Braem S., Notebaert W., Verguts T. (2016). Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. Psychol. Bull. 142 693–728. 10.1037/bul0000047 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Allen T. A., Hallquist M. N. (2020). Disinhibition and detachment in adolescence: a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective on the alternative model for personality disorders. Psychopathology 53 205–212. 10.1159/000509984 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Allport D. A., Styles E. A., Hsieh S. (1994). “Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks,” in Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing, eds Umiltà C., Moscovitch M. (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; ), 421–452.
    1. Arbuthnott K. (2008). The effect of task location and task type on backward inhibition. Mem. Cognit. 36 534–543. 10.3758/mc.36.3.534 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Arbuthnott K., Frank J. (2000). Executive control in set switching: residual switch cost and task-set inhibition. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 54 33–41. 10.1037/h0087328 - DOI - PubMed