Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2020 Dec 11:5:100020.
doi: 10.1016/j.cpnec.2020.100020. eCollection 2021 Feb.

A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of emotion regulation on cortisol

Affiliations
Review

A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of emotion regulation on cortisol

Mai B Mikkelsen et al. Compr Psychoneuroendocrinol. .

Abstract

It is generally acknowledged that hormones are implicated in socioemotional behavior, yet little is known about the role of hormones in the context of emotion regulation. The aims of the present review and meta-analysis were to review and synthesize the available evidence pertaining to the effect of emotion regulation instructions on hormones, and to investigate whether this effect varies according to: type of hormone, context (e.g., emotion-induction procedure), emotion regulation characteristics (e.g., emotion regulation strategy), and presence and type of psychiatric disorder. PubMed, PsycINFO, and CINAHL were searched for experimental studies assessing the effect of instructed emotion regulation on levels of hormones (i.e., testosterone, cortisol, oxytocin, estradiol, and vasopressin) in physically healthy adults. The literature search yielded 17 relevant studies, 16 investigating cortisol and one investigating testosterone. Of these, 12 cortisol studies had eligible data for the meta-analysis. The results of the meta-analysis indicated no statistically significant effect of receiving an emotion regulation instruction compared with receiving no instruction on the cortisol response to subsequent emotion induction (g ​= ​-0.05, p ​= ​.48). However, within-person comparisons of change from an unregulated response to a regulated response indicated a significant change in cortisol levels (g ​= ​0.18, p ​= ​.03) consistent with the specified regulation goal (i.e., either up- or downregulation). No statistically significant effects were found in subgroup meta-analyses conducted according to context, emotion regulation characteristics or psychiatric disorders. Taken together, the findings indicate that emotion-induction procedures are associated with increases in cortisol that may subsequently return to equilibrium regardless of emotion-regulation instructions. Based on the large gaps in research (e.g., few studies investigated other hormones than cortisol, few studies included self-report measures of emotions) identified in the present review, we conclude that the effect of emotion regulation on hormones remains poorly understood.

Prospero registration: CRD42020157336.

Keywords: Behavior; Cortisol; Emotion; Emotion regulation; Hormone.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests: Ms. Mikkelsen, Ms. Tramm, Professor Gravholt and Dr. O’Toole received funding to conduct this research by the VELUX foundation (#26647). Professor Gravholt was also funded by the Novo Nordisk Foundation (NNF13OC0003234, NNF15OC0016474; NNF20OC0060610), the Familien Hede Nielsen foundation and the Independent Research Fund Denmark (0134–00406A). These funders had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
PRISMA flow chart of study selection process. After PRISMA flow diagram [80].
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Forest plot for pooled effect sizes for between-group effects.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Forest plot for pooled effect sizes for within-group effects.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adam E.K., Quinn M.E., Tavernier R., McQuillan M.T., Dahlke K.A., Gilbert K.E. Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;83:25–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Akinola M., Fridman I., Mor S., Morris M.W., Crum A.J. Adaptive appraisals of anxiety moderate the association between cortisol reactivity and performance in salary negotiations. PLoS One. 2016;11(12) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0167977. ∗. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Archer J. Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006;30(3):319–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aldao A., Sheppes G., Gross J.J. Emotion regulation flexibility. Cognit. Ther. Res. 2015;39(3):263–278. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9662-4. - DOI
    1. aan het Rot M., Hogenelst K., Schoevers R.A. Mood disorders in everyday life: a systematic review of experience sampling and ecological momentary assessment studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2012;32(6):510–523. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.05.007. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources