What Is the Weather Prediction Task Good for? A New Analysis of Learning Strategies Reveals How Young Adults Solve the Task
- PMID: 35769734
- PMCID: PMC9234396
- DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.886339
What Is the Weather Prediction Task Good for? A New Analysis of Learning Strategies Reveals How Young Adults Solve the Task
Abstract
The Weather Prediction Task (WPT) was originally designed to assess probabilistic classification learning. Participants were believed to gradually acquire implicit knowledge about cue-outcome association probabilities and solve the task using a multicue strategy based on the combination of all cue-outcome probabilities. However, the cognitive processes engaged in the resolution of this task have not been firmly established, and despite conflicting results, the WPT is still commonly used to assess striatal or procedural learning capacities in various populations. Here, we tested young adults on a modified version of the WPT and performed novel analyses to decipher the learning strategies and cognitive processes that may support above chance performance. The majority of participants used a hierarchical strategy by assigning different weights to the different cues according to their level of predictability. They primarily based their responses on the presence or absence of highly predictive cues and considered less predictive cues secondarily. However, the influence of the less predictive cues was inconsistent with the use of a multicue strategy, since they did not affect choices when both highly predictive cues associated with opposite outcomes were present simultaneously. Our findings indicate that overall performance is inadequate to draw conclusions about the cognitive processes assessed by the WPT. Instead, detailed analyses of performance for the different patterns of cue-outcome associations are essential to determine the learning strategies used by participants to solve the task.
Keywords: conditional learning; explicit; hippocampus; implicit; multiple-cue learning; probabilistic learning; striatum.
Copyright © 2022 Bochud-Fragnière, Banta Lavenex and Lavenex.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Figures




Similar articles
-
When and how do children solve the Weather Prediction Task?Dev Psychobiol. 2023 Sep;65(6):e22407. doi: 10.1002/dev.22407. Dev Psychobiol. 2023. PMID: 37607895
-
Distinguishing the contributions of implicit and explicit processes to performance of the weather prediction task.Mem Cognit. 2009 Mar;37(2):210-22. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.2.210. Mem Cognit. 2009. PMID: 19223570
-
How do people solve the "weather prediction" task?: individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning.Learn Mem. 2002 Nov-Dec;9(6):408-18. doi: 10.1101/lm.45202. Learn Mem. 2002. PMID: 12464701 Free PMC article.
-
Self-insight in probabilistic category learning.J Gen Psychol. 2013 Jan-Mar;140(1):57-81. doi: 10.1080/00221309.2012.735284. J Gen Psychol. 2013. PMID: 24837346
-
Explicit and implicit processes in multicue judgment.Mem Cognit. 2003 Jun;31(4):608-18. doi: 10.3758/bf03196101. Mem Cognit. 2003. PMID: 12872876
Cited by
-
The Visual Advantage Effect in Comparing Uni-Modal and Cross-Modal Probabilistic Category Learning.J Intell. 2023 Nov 27;11(12):218. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence11120218. J Intell. 2023. PMID: 38132836 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Carmer S. G., Swanson M. R. (1973). Evaluation of 10 pairwise multiple comparison procedures by Monte-Carlo methods. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 68, 66–74. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1973.10481335 - DOI
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources