Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Jun 21:16:921802.
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.921802. eCollection 2022.

The Automatic Emotion Regulation of Children Aged 8-12: An ERP Study

Affiliations

The Automatic Emotion Regulation of Children Aged 8-12: An ERP Study

Fang Liu et al. Front Behav Neurosci. .

Abstract

Emotion regulation in childhood and adolescence is related to their social development. Better emotion regulation is associated with great individual academic performance and mental health. However, compared with the research on emotion regulation strategies, children's automatic emotion regulation has been less investigated. Using event-related potential (ERP) technology, this study adopts the cued-emotion Go/Nogo paradigm to investigate the processing characteristics of automatic emotion regulation in children aged 8-12 years. The current study selected 34 younger group [16 boys, 18 girls, mean (M) ± SD = 8.91 ± 0.75], and 31 older group [18 boys, 13 girls, M ± SD = 11.26 ± 0.45]. The results showed that, for Nogo trials, the amplitude of N2 and P3 evoked by emotional faces were significantly larger than those evoked by neutral faces, reflecting the cognitive conflict experienced and the process of children's automatic response inhibition to emotional stimuli, respectively. However, no significant difference in N2 and P3 amplitude were found in Go trials, which may indicate that children aged 8-12 showed similar top-down control and similar motivated attention in this experiment, respectively. Further analysis found that the negative affect of temperament was significantly positively correlated with Nogo-P3 induced by neutral pictures (r = 0.37, p < 0.001), and preadolescents' social anxiety was significantly positively correlated with Nogo-P3 followed by neutral pictures (r = 0.31, p < 0.01). These findings can provide inspiration and empirical support for the promotion and intervention of emotion regulation in children and adolescents.

Keywords: Nogo-P3; automatic emotion regulation; children; social anxiety; temperament.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Schematic representation of the experimental procedure. (A) Nogo trial and (B) Go trial. Facial images sourced from CFAPS database (Wang and Luo, 2005). Reproduced with permission.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Mean reaction time (A) and accuracy (B) were elicited by viewing positive, neutral, and negative faces in the correct Go trials in younger and older children. Error bars denote the standard error (SE). *p < 0.05.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Averaged waveforms (μV) for positive (black lines), neutral (red lines), and negative pictures (blue lines) in Go-Younger (A), Go-Older (B), Nogo-Younger (C), and Nogo-Older (D) conditions. The shadows represent time windows for N2 (200–350 ms) and P3 (350–700 ms).
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Topographic maps of all participants in different conditions.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Scatter plots depicting Pearson’s correlation analyses. Panel (A) shows the positive correlation between Nogo-P3 amplitudes elicited by viewing neutral faces and negative affect measured by the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire-Revised (EATQ-R). Panel (B) shows the positive correlation between Nogo-P3 amplitudes elicited by viewing neutral faces and social anxiety measured by the Social Anxiety Scale For Children (SASC).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ahmed S. P., Bittencourt-Hewitt A., Sebastian C. L. (2015). Neurocognitive bases of emotion regulation development in adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 15 11–25. 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.006 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Albert J., Lopez-Martin S., Carretie L. (2010). Emotional context modulates response inhibition. Neural and behavioral data. NeuroImage 49 914–921. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.045 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Albert J., Lopez-Martin S., Tapia M., Montoya D., Carretie L. (2011). The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in emotional response inhibition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33 2147–2160. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.04510.1002/hbm.21347 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Aldao A., Nolen-Hoeksema S., Schweizer S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30 217–237. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bayle D. J., Taylor M. J. (2010). Attention inhibition of early cortical activation to fearful faces. Brain Res. 1313 113–123. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.060 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources