Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Jul 4;12(7):e9066.
doi: 10.1002/ece3.9066. eCollection 2022 Jul.

Exploring the interplay between natural and intersexual selection on the evolution of a cognitive trait

Affiliations

Exploring the interplay between natural and intersexual selection on the evolution of a cognitive trait

Marie Barou-Dagues et al. Ecol Evol. .

Abstract

There has been an increased focus on the role of natural and sexual selection in shaping cognitive abilities, but the importance of the interaction between both forces remains largely unknown. Intersexual selection through female mate choice might be an important driver of the evolution of cognitive traits, especially in monogamous species, where females may obtain direct fitness benefits by choosing mates with better cognitive abilities. However, the importance given by female to male cognitive traits might vary among species and/or populations according to their life-history traits and ecology. To disentangle the effects of natural and sexual selection, here we use an agent-based simulation model and compare the model's predictions when females mate with the first randomly encountered male (i.e., under natural selection) versus when they choose among males based on their cognitive trait values (i.e., under natural and intersexual selection). Males and females are characterized, respectively, by their problem-solving ability and assessment strategy. At each generation, agents go through (1) a choosing phase during which females assess the cognitive abilities of potential mates until eventually finding an acceptable one and (2) a reproductive phase during which all males compete for limited resources that are exploited at a rate, which depends on their cognitive abilities. Because males provide paternal care, the foraging success of mated males determines the breeding success of the pair through its effect on nestling provisioning efficiency. The model predicts that intersexual selection plays a major role in most ecological conditions, by either reinforcing or acting against the effect of natural selection. The latter case occurs under harsh environmental conditions, where intersexual selection contributes to maintaining cognitive diversity. Our findings thus demonstrate the importance of considering the interaction between both selective forces and highlight the need to build a conceptual framework to target relevant cognitive traits.

Keywords: cognitive performance; intersexual selection; mate choice; problem‐solving ability; provisioning ability.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Overview of the model initialization, simulation processes, and analysis
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Effect of environmental parameters on the pooled distribution of male cognitive trait values at the 50th generation from 100 repetitions, under natural selection alone (left panels) or under natural and sexual selection (right panel). Low and high male cognitive trait values respectively mean that the males have good and low cognitive abilities. (a) Tr = 200, Tm = 200, R = 10, α = 1, and Np is manipulated (b) Tr = 200, Tm = 200, Np = 85, α = 1, and R is manipulated, (c) Tr = 200, Tm = 200, R = 100, Np = 85, and α is manipulated, (d) Tr = 50, Tm = 200, R = 100, Np = 85, and α is manipulated. The purple and yellow colors, respectively, represent low and high values of the manipulated factor, and the gray dashed line represent the initial distribution of the trait
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Effect of environmental parameters on the pooled distribution of female selectivity values at the 50th generation from 100 repetitions, under natural selection alone (left panels) or under natural and sexual selection (right panel). Low and high female selectivity values, respectively, mean that the females have a high and low preference for high male cognitive abilities. (a) Tr = 200, Tm = 200, R = 100, Np = 85, and α is manipulated, (b) Tr = 50, Tm = 200, R = 100, Np = 85, and α is manipulated, (c) Tr = 200, R = 10, Np = 85, α = 1, and Tm is manipulated. The purple and yellow colors, respectively, represent low and high values of the manipulated factor. The patterns found in (a) are similar when the number of patches and the number of items per patch are manipulated, and the gray dashed line represents the initial distribution of the trait
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Mean frequency of male cognitive trait values in the 360 environmental conditions in which we made vary the quantity and quality of food patches, the lengths of the reproductive and choosing phases, and the male cognitive cost factor under natural selection from the last 50 generations over 100 repetitions. Each color represents a male trait value from red (i.e., males with a better cognitive ability) to purple (i.e., males with a poorer cognitive ability)
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Mean frequency of male cognitive trait values in the 360 environmental conditions in which we made vary the quantity and quality of food patches, the lengths of the reproductive, and choosing phases and the male cognitive cost factor under natural and sexual selection from the last 50 generations over 100 repetitions. Each color represents a male trait value from red (i.e., males with a better cognitive ability) to purple (i.e., males with a poorer cognitive ability)

Similar articles

References

    1. Alghamdi, A. , Dalton, L. , Phillis, A. , Rosato, E. , & Mallon, E. B. (2008). Immune response impairs learning in free‐flying bumble‐bees. Biology Letters, 4(5), 479–481. 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0331 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Álvarez‐Quintero, N. , Velando, A. , & Kim, S.‐Y. (2021). Smart mating: the cognitive ability of females influences their preference for male cognitive ability. Behavioral Ecology, 32, 803–813. 10.1093/beheco/arab052 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Arden, R. , Gottfredson, L. S. , Miller, G. , & Pierce, A. (2009). Intelligence and semen quality are positively correlated. Intelligence, 37(3), 277–282. 10.1016/j.intell.2008.11.001 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ashton, B. J. , Thornton, A. , & Ridley, A. R. (2018). An intraspecific appraisal of the social intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1756), 20170288. 10.1098/rstb.2017.0288 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barou‐Dagues, M. , & Dubois, F. (2022). Exploring the interplay between natural and intersexual selection on the evolution of a cognitive trait. Dryad, Dataset, 10.5061/dryad.bcc2fqzfx - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources