Cognitive segmentation and fluid reasoning in childhood
- PMID: 35848224
- PMCID: PMC7614553
- DOI: 10.1177/17470218221116054
Cognitive segmentation and fluid reasoning in childhood
Abstract
The ability to solve novel complex problems predicts success in a wide range of areas. Recent research suggests that the ability to cognitively segment complex problems into smaller parts constrains nonverbal reasoning in adults. This study aimed to test whether cognitively segmenting problems improves nonverbal reasoning performance for children as it does for adults. A total of 115 children aged 6-10 years completed two versions of a modified traditional matrix reasoning task in which demands on working memory, integration, and processing speed were minimised, such that the only significant requirement was to break each problem into its constituent parts. In one version of the task, participants were presented with a traditional 2×2 Matrix and asked to draw the missing matrix item into a response box below. In a second version, the problem was broken down into its component features across three separate cells, reducing the need for participants to segment the problem. As with adults, performance was better in the condition in which the problems were separated into component parts. Children with lower fluid intelligence did not benefit more in the separated condition than children with higher fluid intelligence, and there was no evidence that segmenting problems was more beneficial for younger than older children. This study demonstrates that cognitive segmentation is a critical component of complex problem-solving for children, as it is for adults. By forcing children to focus their attention on separate parts of a complex visual problem, their performance can be dramatically improved.
Keywords: Cognitive segmentation; analogical reasoning; fluid intelligence; matrix reasoning; problem-solving.
Conflict of interest statement
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Figures




Similar articles
-
Complexity and compositionality in fluid intelligence.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017 May 16;114(20):5295-5299. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1621147114. Epub 2017 May 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017. PMID: 28461462 Free PMC article.
-
Working Memory and Reasoning Benefit from Different Modes of Large-scale Brain Dynamics in Healthy Older Adults.J Cogn Neurosci. 2018 Jul;30(7):1033-1046. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01260. Epub 2018 Mar 21. J Cogn Neurosci. 2018. PMID: 29561235
-
Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: a controlled, randomized study.Dev Sci. 2011 May;14(3):591-601. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01022.x. Dev Sci. 2011. PMID: 21477197 Clinical Trial.
-
How does anxiety influence fluid reasoning?Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2017 Jul-Sep;6(3):248-254. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2017.1317493. Epub 2017 May 12. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2017. PMID: 28497992 Review.
-
Cognitive and psychomotor effects of risperidone in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder.Clin Ther. 2008 Sep;30(9):1565-89. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2008.09.014. Clin Ther. 2008. PMID: 18840365 Review.
References
-
- Aslan D., Arnas Y. A. (2007). Three-to six-year-old children’s recognition of geometric shapes. International Journal of Early Years Education, 15(1), 83–104. 10.1080/09669760601106646 - DOI
-
- Bethell-Fox C. E., Lohman D. F., Snow R. E. (1984). Adaptive reasoning: Componential and eye movement analysis of geometric analogy performance. Intelligence, 8(3), 205–238. 10.1016/0160-2896(84)90009-6 - DOI
-
- Cattell R. B. (1940). A culture free intelligence test, part I. Journal of Educational Psychology, 31, 161–169.
MeSH terms
Grants and funding
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Research Materials