Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Jul 28;12(1):12890.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-16198-z.

The psychophysiological mechanisms of real-world time experience

Affiliations

The psychophysiological mechanisms of real-world time experience

Ruth S Ogden et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Our sense of time is fallible, often resulting in the sensation of time flying by quickly or dragging slowly. It has been suggested that changes in sympathetic (SNS) and parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) activity may influence the perceived passage of time, however this proposition has never been tested during real-world temporal experience. The current study directly tested the relationship between the passage of time and SNS-PNS activity in the real-world. Sixty-seven participants completed a normal day's activities whilst wearing sensors to capture electrocardiography (ECG), electrodermal activity (EDA) and movement. They also provided hourly rating of the subjective speed at which time was passing. Results revealed that greater SNS activity (e.g., increased heart rate, frequency of phasic skin conductance response) was associated with time passing more quickly. PNS activity was not related to time experience. Whilst the findings support previous suggestions that changes in physiological arousal are associated with distortions to the passage of time, the effects are small and other factors are likely to contribute to real-world temporal experience.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Line plots with error bars for standardised subjective data, (a) passage of time judgements (b) mood, and (c) engagement. Higher scores = time passing quickly (a), positive mood (b), and very engaged (c).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Descriptive statistics for standardised residuals for Heart Rate (HR) (a) and RMSSD (b). Red line = mean, pink box = standard deviation, blue lines = 95% confidence intervals, grey circles = raw data.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Descriptive statistics for standardised residuals from Skin Conductance Level (SCL) (a) and frequency of Skin Conductance Response (SCR) (b). Red line = mean, pink box = standard deviation, blue lines = 95% confidence intervals, grey circles = raw data.

References

    1. Mella N, Conty L, Pouthas V. The role of physiological arousal in time perception: Psychophysiological evidence from an emotion regulation paradigm. Brain Cogn. 2011;75:182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.012. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ogden RS. The passage of time during the UK Covid-19 lockdown. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0235871. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235871. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ogden R. Distortions to the passage of time during England’s second national lockdown: A role for depression. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0250412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250412. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Wearden J, O’Donoghue A, Ogden R, Montgomery C. Subjective duration in the laboratory and the world outside. In: Arstila V, Lloyd D, editors. Subjective Time: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality. MIT Press; 2014. p. 287.
    1. Whillans AV, Dunn EW, Smeets P, Bekkers R, Norton MI. Buying time promotes happiness. PNAS. 2017;114:8523–8527. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1706541114. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types