Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2021 Dec;13(12):2323.
doi: 10.3390/sym13122323. Epub 2021 Dec 4.

Temporal Dynamics of Event-Related Potentials during Inhibitory Control Characterize Age-Related Neural Compensation

Affiliations

Temporal Dynamics of Event-Related Potentials during Inhibitory Control Characterize Age-Related Neural Compensation

Elizabeth R Paitel et al. Symmetry (Basel). 2021 Dec.

Abstract

Aging is accompanied by frontal lobe and non-dominant hemisphere recruitment that supports executive functioning, such as inhibitory control, which is crucial to all cognitive functions. However, the spatio-temporal sequence of processing underlying successful inhibition and how it changes with age is understudied. Thus, we capitalized on the temporal precision of event-related potentials (ERPs) to assess the functional lateralization of N200 (conflict monitoring) and P300 (inhibitory performance evaluation) in young and healthy older adults during comparably performed successful stop-signal inhibition. We additionally used temporal principal components analysis (PCA) to further interrogate the continuous spatio-temporal dynamics underlying N200 and P300 activation for each group. Young adults demonstrated left hemisphere-dominant N200, while older adults demonstrated overall larger amplitudes and right hemisphere dominance. N200 activation was explained by a single PCA factor in both age groups, but with a more anterior scalp distribution in older adults. The P300 amplitudes were larger in the right hemisphere in young, but bilateral in old, with old larger than young in the left hemisphere. P300 was also explained by a single factor in young adults but by two factors in older adults, including distinct parieto-occipital and anterior activation. These findings highlight the differential functional asymmetries of conflict monitoring (N200) and inhibitory evaluation and adaptation (P300) processes and further illuminate unique age-related spatio-temporal recruitment patterns. Older adults demonstrated lateralized recruitment during conflict processing and bilateral recruitment during evaluation and adaptation, with anterior recruitment common to both processes. These fine-grained analyses are critically important for more precise understanding of age-related compensatory activation.

Keywords: N200; P300; cognitive aging; electroencephalography; event-related potentials; executive function; inhibitory control; neural recruitment.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Average ERP amplitude (μV) ± SEM is shown by age group at left (electrode 3) and right (electrode 4) hemisphere sites for frontal through parietal regions (F = frontal; FC = fronto-central; C = central; P = parietal) for N200 (left column) and P300 (right column) components. For N200, a negative-going wave, larger amplitudes are negative (i.e., downward). For P300, a positive-going wave, larger amplitudes are positive (i.e., upward). Corresponding significant group differences are specified in Table 3 and mean and SEM provided in Table 4.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
A) PCA factor loadings by group. B-D) 64-channel grand average ERPs (one tracing/electrode). Electrodes with significant factor loadings shown in color; scalp maps show spatial distributions. Young adults (left): Factor 1) central-parietal positivity (F1P; P300-related; B1); Factor 2 was a parietal-occipital negativity (F2N; N200-related; C1). Older adults (right): Factor 1) two activation phases, with a central negative peak (F1N; N200-related) and a frontal-central positive peak (F1P; P300-related), both of which were diffuse (anterior to posterior extent), with more anterior maximum than Young; Factor 2) two positive parietal-occipital peaks reflecting added visual attention/ processing (F2P); Factor 3) early right frontal positivity (F3P; e.g., P200, sensory-perceptual processing).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Salthouse TA When Does Age-Related Cognitive Decline Begin? Neurobiol. Aging 2009, 30, 507–514, doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Verhaeghen P; Salthouse TA Meta-Analyses of age–Cognition Relations in Adulthood: Estimates of Linear and Nonlinear Age Effects and Structural Models. Psychol. Bull 1997, 122, 231–249. - PubMed
    1. Deary IJ; Corley J; Gow AJ; Harris SE; Houlihan LM; Marioni RE; Penke L; Rafnsson SB; Starr JM Age-Associated Cognitive Decline. Br. Med. Bull 2009, 92, 135–152, doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldp033. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Munakata Y; Herd SA; Chatham CH; Depue BE; Banich MT; O’Reilly RC A Unified Framework for Inhibitory Control. Trends Cognit. Sci 2011, 15, 453–459, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.011. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. West R; Alain C Age-Related Decline in Inhibitory Control Contributes to the Increased Stroop Effect Observed in Older Adults. Psychophysiology 2000, 37, 179–189, doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720179. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources