Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2022 Oct:316:114776.
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114776. Epub 2022 Aug 7.

The relationship between novelty-seeking traits and behavior: Establishing construct validity for the human Behavioral Pattern Monitor

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

The relationship between novelty-seeking traits and behavior: Establishing construct validity for the human Behavioral Pattern Monitor

Arpi Minassian et al. Psychiatry Res. 2022 Oct.

Abstract

Novelty seeking is a tendency to approach new situations, putatively driven by the brain's catecholaminergic system. It is traditionally measured via self-report, but a laboratory-based paradigm, the human Behavioral Pattern Monitor (hBPM), quantifies behavior in a novel environment and has utility in cross-species studies of neuropsychiatric disorders. Our primary aim assessed whether self-reported novelty-seeking traits were associated with novelty-seeking behavior in the hBPM. An existing sample of 106 volunteers were categorized as high vs. low novelty seekers using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). Subjects had been randomized to one dose of amphetamine (10 or 20 mg) or modafinil (200 or 400 mg), allowing us to explore whether a pharmacological catecholamine challenge further enhanced novelty-seeking behavior. High TCI novelty-seekers had more hBPM motor activity and novel object interactions. The exploratory analyses, although limited by low power, suggested that amphetamine and modafinil did not markedly moderate novelty-seeking traits. The hBPM demonstrates construct validity as a lab-based measure of novelty seeking and thus useful in translational studies of neuropsychiatric conditions and treatment options. Further research may illuminate whether a biological predisposition towards higher catecholaminergic activity, combined with the novelty-seeking trait, may increase propensity for risky and addictive behaviors.

Keywords: Amphetamine; Exploration; Modafinil; Motor activity; Temperament and character inventory.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of Competing Interest The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
The human Behavioral Pattern Monitor (hBPM)
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Participant Flow Diagram.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Higher novelty-seeking scores on the TCI are associated with higher motor activity in the hBPM as measured by acceleration (Fig 3a) and higher number of hBPM novel object interactions (Fig 3b).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Novelty seeking on the TCI is not strongly associated with catecholamine agonist drug effects in the hBPM as measured by acceleration (Figure 4a) and number of hBPM novel object interactions (Figure 4b). Figure legend: # High novelty-seekers > low novelty-seekers, p < .10.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Scatterplot of correlation between TCI novelty-seeking score and motor activity in the hBPM as measured by acceleration (Fig 5A), and TCI novelty-seeking score and hBPM novel object interactions (Fig 5B).
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Scatterplot of correlation between TCI novelty-seeking score and motor activity in the hBPM as measured by acceleration (Fig 5A), and TCI novelty-seeking score and hBPM novel object interactions (Fig 5B).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ballard ME, Gallo DA, de Wit H, 2014. Amphetamine increases errors during episodic memory retrieval. J Clin Psychopharmacol 34 (1), 85–92. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bunzeck N, Duzel E, 2006. Absolute coding of stimulus novelty in the human substantia nigra/VTA. Neuron 51 (3), 369–379. - PubMed
    1. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, Munafo MR, 2013. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14 (5), 365–376. - PubMed
    1. Cloninger CR, Przybeck T, Svrakic D, Wetzel R, 1994. The Temperament and Character Inventory: a guide to its development and use. Washington University, Center for Psychobiology of Personality, St. Louis.
    1. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR, 1993. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50 (12), 975–990. - PubMed

Publication types