Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Aug 23:16:884080.
doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.884080. eCollection 2022.

Cannabis use is associated with sexually dimorphic changes in executive control of visuospatial decision-making

Affiliations

Cannabis use is associated with sexually dimorphic changes in executive control of visuospatial decision-making

Parker J Banks et al. Front Integr Neurosci. .

Abstract

When the outcome of a choice is less favorable than expected, humans and animals typically shift to an alternate choice option on subsequent trials. Several lines of evidence indicate that this "lose-shift" responding is an innate sensorimotor response strategy that is normally suppressed by executive function. Therefore, the lose-shift response provides a covert gauge of cognitive control over choice mechanisms. We report here that the spatial position, rather than visual features, of choice targets drives the lose-shift effect. Furthermore, the ability to inhibit lose-shift responding to gain reward is different among male and female habitual cannabis users. Increased self-reported cannabis use was concordant with suppressed response flexibility and an increased tendency to lose-shift in women, which reduced performance in a choice task in which random responding is the optimal strategy. On the other hand, increased cannabis use in men was concordant with reduced reliance on spatial cues during decision-making, and had no impact on the number of correct responses. These data (63,600 trials from 106 participants) provide strong evidence that spatial-motor processing is an important component of economic decision-making, and that its governance by executive systems is different in men and women who use cannabis frequently.

Keywords: addiction; cannabis; choice; executive control; habit; lose-shift; sex differences; spatial processing.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Behavioral task. (A) Timeline of trials in the matching pennies game. (B) Reconfiguration of targets, which could undergo local (swap and displace) and global changes in position.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Effect of recreational drug use on measures of task performance in males and females: proportion of wins (A); response entropy (B); and decision times (C). Plots show the conventional descriptive statistics: mean (diamond), median (horizontal line in the box), 25th/75th percentiles (box edges), and outliers (dots). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Effects of target reconfiguration and recreational drug use on reinforcement-driven behavior. (A,B) Effect of local and global changes in choice position on lose-shift and win-stay tendencies for all participants. SEM in error bars. (C,D) Box plots of the difference in lose-shift and win stay when target positions are swapped as compared to no change. The effect of swapping targets on lose-shift is higher in women who use cannabis, but lower in men who use cannabis, than their sex-matched controls. (E) Correlation between total drug use (ASSIST) and swap effect on lose-shift. *p < 0.05.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Effect of local and global changes in choice position on lose-shift tendencies in male and female cannabis users, relative to controls. SEM in error bars.
Figure 5
Figure 5
(A) Performance of the Q-learning (Q), Q-learning with forgetting (FQ), and Q-learning with differential forgetting (DFQ) models. (B) Relationship between κ1 and win-stay behavior with curves fit to individual subjects (blue lines), the population average (black line), and averages of binned raw data (points). (C,D) Relationship between κ2 & α and lose-shift behavior. α was normalized via the logit transform prior to model fitting.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Effect of wins, local, and global changes in choice position on Q-learning parameters α (A) (learning rate), κ1 (B) (reward strength), and κ2 (C) (punishment strength). The influence of wins and cue rearrangement during the previous 32 trials is estimated by Volterra decomposition, which provides a weight (loading) for each trial lag. SEM in shaded area.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Cannabis × Sex interactions on mean reinforcement learning parameter values estimated by Volterra decomposition. Learning rate α (A) and effect of wins κ1 (B) to wins, local displacement, swaps, and global changes. SEM in error bars. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Similar articles

References

    1. Abouzari M., Oberg S., Gruber A., Tata M. (2015). Interactions among attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and problem gambling in a probabilistic reward-learning task. Behav. Brain Res. 291, 237–243. 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.041 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Albrecht D. S., Skosnik P. D., Vollmer J. M., Brumbaugh M. S., Perry K. M., Mock B. H., et al. . (2013). Striatal D2/D3 receptor availability is inversely correlated with cannabis consumption in chronic marijuana users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 128, 52–57. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.07.016 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Balleine B. W., O'Doherty J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 48–69. 10.1038/npp.2009.131 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Banks P. J., Tata M. S., Bennett P. J., Sekuler A. B., Gruber A. J. (2018). Implicit valuation of the near-miss is dependent on outcome context. J. Gambl. Stud. 34, 181–197. 10.1007/s10899-017-9705-3 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barr D. J., Levy R., Scheepers C., Tily H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 255–278. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources