Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Oct 25:11:e78162.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.78162.

Neural defensive circuits underlie helping under threat in humans

Affiliations

Neural defensive circuits underlie helping under threat in humans

Joana B Vieira et al. Elife. .

Abstract

Empathy for others' distress has long been considered the driving force of helping. However, when deciding to help others in danger, one must consider not only their distress, but also the risk to oneself. Whereas the role of self-defense in helping has been overlooked in human research, studies in other animals indicate defensive responses are necessary for the protection of conspecifics. In this pre-registered study (N=49), we demonstrate that human defensive neural circuits are implicated in helping others under threat. Participants underwent fMRI scanning while deciding whether to help another participant avoid aversive electrical shocks, at the risk of also being shocked. We found that higher engagement of neural circuits that coordinate fast escape from self-directed danger (including the insula, PAG, and ACC) facilitated decisions to help others. Importantly, using representational similarity analysis, we found that the strength with which the amygdala and insula uniquely represented the threat to oneself (and not the other's distress) predicted helping. Our findings indicate that in humans, as other mammals, defensive mechanisms play a greater role in helping behavior than previously understood.

Keywords: Brain; Fear; altruism; evolutionary biology; human; neuroscience.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

JV, AO No competing interests declared

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Outline of the experimental tasks.
(A) fMRI helping under threat task. Participants saw the co-participant on the screen, together with a visual cue signaling threat (an upcoming shock). There were three threat levels: safe (0 shocks, green circle), moderate threat (1 shock, yellow circle), and high threat (2 shocks, red circle). In each trial of the task, the circle started static on the left (4 s), and then moved to the right (4 s). Participants were prompted to decide whether they wanted to help the co-participant or not (1.25–1.75 s) either in the beginning of trial (distal) or right before the moment of shock delivery (imminent). Therefore, the available time to make a decision was identical in distal and imminent threats. If participants decided to help, there was a 70% chance both themselves and co-participant would receive shocks; if they decided not to help, the co-participant would always receive a shock, and the participant would not. Decisions prompted on safe trials were to arbitrarily choose to press 1 or 2, since no shocks would be administered. (B) After the fMRI task, outside the scanner, participants re-watched clips of the co-participant presented during the scan, and were asked to rate how much ‘discomfort, anxiety or uneasiness’ he was experiencing in each clip on a 9-point scale. They also presented images of the threat cues and asked to rate, on the same scale, how threatened they felt themselves when they saw those images during the scan.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Behavioural results.
(A). There was no evidence of differential helping during imminent and distal threats, nor during 1-shock and 2-shock trials. (B) Difference between proportion of helping in imminent and distal trials (y axis) across subjects (x axis); 27 participants helped more during imminent than distal threats, 11 helped more during distal, and 11 helped the same amount. (C) Responses were faster during imminent than distal trials across threat levels. (D) Participants rated the co-participant’s distress as higher during imminent than distal trials, and as progressively higher across the three threat levels. (E) Participants reported feeling more threatened when watching 2-shock cues (red circle), followed by 1-shock cues (yellow circle), and safe cues (green circle), and when watching cues signaling imminent than distal threat.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Combined ROI mask including bilateral ventral and lateral medial frontal cortex, dorsal ACC, insula, hippocampus, amygdala, and midbrain.
ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; ROI, region of interest.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.. Multivariate and univariate fMRI results.
(A) Local multivoxel activation patterns (identified by searchlight analysis) in the insula and dmPFC were distinguishable between distal and imminent threats, irrespective of helping decisions. (B) Local multivoxel activation patterns (identified by support vector regression) in the amygdala, insula, OFC/IFG, vmPFC, and ACC were linearly associated with varying threat level. (C) Local multivoxel activation patterns (identified by searchlight) in the insula, IFG, hippocampus, and ACC were distinguishable when making helping decisions under distal and imminent threat. (D) Clusters in the insula, IFG, OFC, and ACC displayed a significant threat imminence*threat level interaction. (E) Clusters in the insula, ACC, IFG/OFC, vmPFC, hippocampus, and PAG displayed a significant type of decision*threat imminence interaction. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AMY, amygdala; dmPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; Hipp, hippocampus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; Ins, insula; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PAG, periaqueductal gray; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.. In the left and middle panels, comparison of ANOVA and parametric modulation activation maps.
Since results of these two analyses suggested the effects were driven by the distal condition, here we selected the distal help versus no help (no help+safe) contrast from the ANOVA (left) and the help and no help maps from the parametric modulator regressors. Commonalities were found in the insula (activation associated with helping decisions) and in the vmPFC (associated with not helping decisions). Red denotes higher activation during help decisions, and blue denotes higher activation during not help decisions. In the right panel, results of the Bayesian model selection (BMS; following Bayesian first-level analysis). Resulting model evidence maps were thresholded at 0.75 (BF of approx. 8). ROI masks were then applied for model comparison. Results showed stronger evidence for help models for the insula, and no help models for the vmPFC, in line with the frequentist analyses. Note that results from the parametric modulation and Bayesian analysis are inherently noisier, given the smaller number of participants and trials. ROI, region of interest; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
Figure 6.
Figure 6.. Regardless of threat imminence, the similarity between neural and threat RDMs in the left amygdala and insula predicted higher frequency of helping decisions.
*p=0.047; **p=0.006. RDM, representational dissimilarity matrix.
Figure 7.
Figure 7.. Schematic of the RSA pipeline.
On step 1, we extracted the vector of trial-by-trial betas for each voxel in a given ROI. We then calculated the correlation (Pearson r) between all trial pairs. These correlation values were inverted (1−r) and used to create a trial-by-trial matrix, wherein each cell represents how correlated activation across all voxels of the ROI was in each trial pair (neural representational dissimilarity matrix, RDM). On step 2, post-scan ratings of the co-participant’s distress in each unique clip were used to construct a trial-by-trial matrix, wherein each cell contained the Euclidean distance between the rating of each pair of clips (distress RDM). A similar method was used with the ratings of threat to the participant (threat RDM). On step 3, the second-order similarity between the neural RDM and distress RDM, and between the neural RDM and threat RDM were calculated using a ranked correlation method (Kendall’s tau). ROI, region of interest; RSA, representational similarity analysis.
Appendix 1—figure 1.
Appendix 1—figure 1.. Example RDMs (Neural, Distress and Threat) sorted by threat level.
Appendix 1—figure 2.
Appendix 1—figure 2.. Response distributions per fMRI run.
Appendix 1—figure 3.
Appendix 1—figure 3.. Cross-validation.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allen HN, Bobnar HJ, Kolber BJ. Left and right hemispheric lateralization of the amygdala in pain. Progress in Neurobiology. 2021;196:101891. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101891. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baas D, Aleman A, Kahn RS. Lateralization of amygdala activation: a systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews. 2004;45:96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.02.004. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Batson CD, Fultz J, Schoenrade PA. Distress and empathy: two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. Journal of Personality. 1987;55:19–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00426.x. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ben-Ami Bartal I, Shan H, Molasky NMR, Murray TM, Williams JZ, Decety J, Mason P. Anxiolytic treatment impairs helping behavior in rats. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:850. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00850. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bernhardt BC, Singer T. The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2012;35:1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150536. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types