Rapid sleep education: If you could tell people one thing about sleep, what should it be?
- PMID: 36325762
- DOI: 10.1111/jsr.13765
Rapid sleep education: If you could tell people one thing about sleep, what should it be?
Abstract
Informal learning settings such as museums provide unique opportunities for educating a local community about sleep. However, in such settings, information must be capable of immediately inciting interest. We developed a series of sleep "icebreakers" (brief, informal facts) to determine whether they elicited interest in sleep and encouraged behavioural change. There were 859 participants across three cross-sectional samples: (a) members of the local museum; (b) Mechanical Turk workers who responded to a "sleep" study advertisement; and (c) Mechanical Turk workers who responded to a "various topics" study advertisement that did not mention sleep. All three samples demonstrated high interest in sleep topics, though delayed recall of the icebreakers was strongest in participants who expected to learn about the sleep topics. Icebreaker interest ratings were independent of age, gender and race/ethnicity, suggesting that sleep is a topic of universal interest. Importantly, regardless of demographics and sample, the more the icebreakers interested the participants, the more likely participants were to indicate willingness to donate to a sleep exhibit, change their sleep behaviours, and post to social media. Thus, sleep icebreakers can rapidly elicit people's interest, and future outreach efforts should couple icebreakers with opportunities for subsequent personalized learning.
Keywords: behavioural change; informal science education; intentions; museums; public engagement; science outreach.
© 2022 The Authors. Journal of Sleep Research published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Sleep Research Society.
References
REFERENCES
-
- Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, white women. Health Psychology, 19(6), 586-592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586
-
- Baron, K. G., Abbott, S., Jao, N., Manalo, N., & Mullen, R. (2017). Orthosomnia: Are some patients taking the quantified self too far? Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 13(2), 351-354. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.6472
-
- Bastien, C. H., Vallières, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2(4), 297-307. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4
-
- Dilenschneider, C. (2017). People trust museums more than newspapers. Here is why that matters right now (DATA). Colleen Dilenschneider. https://www.colleendilen.com/2017/04/26/people-trust-museums-more-than-n...
-
- Dunning, D. (2011). The Dunning-Kruger effect: On being ignorant of one's own ignorance. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 247-296). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-385522-0.00005-6
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources