Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Nov 3;12(11):1494.
doi: 10.3390/brainsci12111494.

Well Played! Promoting Phonemic Awareness Training Using EdTech-GraphoGame Brazil-During the COVID-19 Pandemic

Affiliations

Well Played! Promoting Phonemic Awareness Training Using EdTech-GraphoGame Brazil-During the COVID-19 Pandemic

Juliana G Marques de Souza et al. Brain Sci. .

Abstract

Early literacy skills such as alphabet knowledge and phonemic awareness are made up the foundation for learning to read. These skills are more effectively taught with explicit instruction starting inpreschool and then continuing during early elementary school years. The COVID19 pandemic school closures severely impacted early literacy development worldwide. Brazil had one of the longest school closure periods, which resulted in several children having no access to any educational activities. Education Technology (EdTech) tools can leverage access to pedagogical materials and remediate the consequences of school closure. We investigated the impact of using an early literacy EdTech, GraphoGame Brazil, to foster learning of early literacy skills during the height of COVID19 school closures, in Brazil. We carried out a quasi-experimental, pretest and posttest study with elementary school students who were taking online classes. Participants were pseudo randomly assigned to (1) an experimental group, who played GraphoGame Brazil, and to (2) an active control group, who played an EdTech that focuses on early numeracy skills. The results show a significant positive training effect on word reading accuracy associated with the use of GraphoGame for the children in the experimental group, relative to the control group. We also found statistically significant negative effect in lowercase naming for the control group. We address the consequences of COVID19 school closures, the promise of EdTech and its limitations, and discuss the issue of fostering successful early literacy instruction in countries that have struggled with teaching children to read even before the pandemic.

Keywords: EdTech; GraphoGame; early literacy; phonemic awareness; school closure.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Figures

Figure A1
Figure A1
Experimental group test scores for lowercase naming accuracy and efficiency, uppercase naming accuracy and efficiency, with scores varying in a range from 0 to 2 letters per second and 0 to 50 correctly recognized letters; word reading accuracy, with scores ranging from 0 to 36, and word reading efficiency, with scores measuring words per second; phonemic awareness scores, ranging in a scale from 0 to 16 (moderate effect). Note: significant difference between test times means for the cited conditions presented in *.
Figure A1
Figure A1
Experimental group test scores for lowercase naming accuracy and efficiency, uppercase naming accuracy and efficiency, with scores varying in a range from 0 to 2 letters per second and 0 to 50 correctly recognized letters; word reading accuracy, with scores ranging from 0 to 36, and word reading efficiency, with scores measuring words per second; phonemic awareness scores, ranging in a scale from 0 to 16 (moderate effect). Note: significant difference between test times means for the cited conditions presented in *.
Figure A1
Figure A1
Experimental group test scores for lowercase naming accuracy and efficiency, uppercase naming accuracy and efficiency, with scores varying in a range from 0 to 2 letters per second and 0 to 50 correctly recognized letters; word reading accuracy, with scores ranging from 0 to 36, and word reading efficiency, with scores measuring words per second; phonemic awareness scores, ranging in a scale from 0 to 16 (moderate effect). Note: significant difference between test times means for the cited conditions presented in *.
Figure 1
Figure 1
Intervention design: active control and experimental groups.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Experimental group test scores for: (a) uppercase naming accuracy, scores ranging from 0 to 50 (moderate effect size); (b) word reading accuracy, scores ranging from 0 to 36 (strong effect size) (c) phonemic and phonological awareness scores, scores ranging in a scale from 0 to 16 (moderate effect). Note: significant difference between test times means for the cited conditions (highlighted by *). Graphs for all conditions along with a side-by-side comparison are provided in the Appendix A (See Figure A1).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Control group test scores for: (a) lowercase naming accuracy, scores ranging from 0 to 50 (moderate negative effect size); (b) phonemic and phonological awareness scores, scores ranging from 0 to 16 (moderate effect). Note: significant difference between test times means for the cited conditions (highlighted by *). Graphs for all conditions along with a side-by-side comparison are provided in the Appendix A (See Figure A1).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Experimental and active control groups letter naming (NAR) accuracy score mean (a) lowercase letter naming accuracy; (b) uppercase naming accuracy. Note: significant difference between group means for both uppercase and lowercase naming accuracy (* β = 3.81735, x2 < 0.03).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Word reading accuracy and efficiency means (a) TDE accuracy for both groups; (b) TDE efficiency for both groups. Significant training effect for word reading accuracy in the experimental group (* β = 1.4681, x2 < 0.04).
Figure 6
Figure 6
Scatterplots of the correlation between the dyslexia screener scale and the dependent variables (a) word reading accuracy; (b) word reading efficiency; (c) uppercase reading accuracy; (d) uppercase reading efficiency; (e) lowercase naming efficiency. Note: * = p < 0.05.

Similar articles

References

    1. Greenough W.T., Black J.E., Wallace C.S. Experience and brain development. Child Dev. 1987;58:539–559. doi: 10.2307/1130197. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Dehaene S. How We Learn: Why Brains Learn Better Than Any Machine... for Now. Volume 370 Penguin; London, UK: 2021.
    1. Dehaene S. Reading in the Brain: The Science and Evolution of a Human Invention. Viking; New York, NY, USA: 2009.
    1. Buchweitz A., Mason R.A., Tomitch L.M.B., Just M.A. Brain activation for reading and listening comprehension: An fMRI study of modality effects and individual differences in language comprehension. Psychol. Neurosci. 2009;2:111–123. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.003. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Dehaene S., Cohen L., Morais J., Kolinsky R. Illiterate to literate: Behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:234–244. doi: 10.1038/nrn3924. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources