Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Nov 11;12(1):19281.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-23716-6.

The role of hand size in body representation: a developmental investigation

Affiliations

The role of hand size in body representation: a developmental investigation

Dorothy Cowie et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Knowledge of one's own body size is a crucial facet of body representation, both for acting on the environment and perhaps also for constraining body ownership. However, representations of body size may be somewhat plastic, particularly to allow for physical growth in childhood. Here we report a developmental investigation into the role of hand size in body representation (the sense of body ownership, perception of hand position, and perception of own-hand size). Using the rubber hand illusion paradigm, this study used different fake hand sizes (60%, 80%, 100%, 120% or 140% of typical size) in three age groups (6- to 7-year-olds, 12- to 13-year-olds, and adults; N = 229). We found no evidence that hand size constrains ownership or position: participants embodied hands which were both larger and smaller than their own, and indeed judged their own hands to have changed size following the illusion. Children and adolescents embodied the fake hands more than adults, with a greater tendency to feel their own hand had changed size. Adolescents were particularly sensitive to multisensory information. In sum, we found substantial plasticity in the representation of own-body size, with partial support for the hypothesis that children have looser representations than adults.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Procedure. Baseline: eyes closed, pointing under table to hand (inset). Illusion: stroking with brushes. Post-illusion pointing: eyes closed, pointing under table to hand (inset). Illusion & post-illusion points repeat (see “Methods”). Affordance: judging the size of an aperture the hand can fit through. Questions (see “Methods”). Fake hand for illustration only—see Fig. 2 for actual hands used.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Fake hands. To illustrate the appearance of the fake hands, this figure shows the set used for 6–7-year-olds. A corresponding second set was used for the other two groups.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Touch referral. This figure shows the medians and interquartile ranges of ratings on the questionnaire item regarding touch referral to the fake hand. Ratings are shown for each age group, fake hand size, and condition.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Ownership. This figure shows the medians and interquartile ranges of ratings on the questionnaire item regarding ownership of the fake hand. Ratings are shown for each age group, fake hand size, and condition.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Hand felt larger. This figure shows the medians and interquartile ranges of ratings on the questionnaire item regarding one’s own hand feeling larger following the illusion. Ratings are shown for each age group, fake hand size, and condition.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Hand felt smaller. This figure shows the medians and interquartile ranges of ratings on the questionnaire item regarding one’s own hand feeling smaller following the illusion. Ratings are shown for each age group, fake hand size, and condition.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Affordance judgment. This figure shows the means and standard errors of affordance judgments, for each age group, fake hand size, and condition.
Figure 8
Figure 8
Proprioceptive drift. This figure shows the mean and standard errors of the proprioceptive drift measure, for each age group, fake hand size, and condition.

References

    1. Blanke O. Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13(8):556–571. doi: 10.1038/nrn3292. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Tsakiris M, Haggard P. The rubber hand illusion revisited: Visuotactile integration and self-attribution. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2005;31(1):80–91. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.80. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Pavani F, Zampini M. The role of hand size in the fake-hand illusion paradigm. Perception. 2007;36(10):1547–1554. doi: 10.1068/p5853. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Cardinali L, Serino A, Gori M. Hand size underestimation grows during childhood. Sci. Rep. 2019;9(1):1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49500-7. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cowie D, Makin TR, Bremner AJ. Children’s responses to the rubber-hand illusion reveal dissociable pathways in body representation. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24(5):762–769. doi: 10.1177/0956797612462902. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types