Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Jul 27:7:23969415221115045.
doi: 10.1177/23969415221115045. eCollection 2022 Jan-Dec.

Autistic preschoolers' engagement and language use in gross motor versus symbolic play settings

Affiliations

Autistic preschoolers' engagement and language use in gross motor versus symbolic play settings

Amanda V Binns et al. Autism Dev Lang Impair. .

Abstract

Background and aims: Although adjustment of the environment is recommended as a support strategy in evidence-based interventions for children with autism, the impact of doing so (and the how and why) is not well understood. One essential environmental factor to consider when providing supports for preschool-aged autistic children is the play setting, specifically, the materials available in the child's play context. The aim of this study was to compare engagement states and number of utterances produced by preschool-aged autistic children within symbolic vs. gross motor play settings. Examining the relationship between gross motor play settings and children's social engagement and spoken language use is particularly important to explore for autistic children given differences in their sensory processing, motor skill development, and choice of and interaction with toys relative to neurotypical peers. Methods: Seventy autistic children aged 25-57 months were videotaped during natural play interactions with a parent. Children's social engagement and number of spoken utterances were examined in five minutes each of play with symbolic toys and play with gross motor toys. Continuous time-tagged video coding of the child-caregiver engagement states was conducted, and the child's frequency of spoken language was identified using language sample analysis. The specific variables examined were; (a) engagement with caregiver, (b) engagement with objects only, (c) unengaged (no evident engagement with objects or people), and (d) total number of spoken utterances. The relationship between play setting (symbolic vs gross motor) and child language and engagement state variables was examined with linear mixed effects modelling. Results: Significant main effects were revealed for the interaction between play setting and autistic children's engagement. Young autistic children were more likely to engage with caregivers in play environments with gross motor toys (moderate effect) and also were more likely to have periods of unengaged time (not overtly directing their attention to objects or people; small effect) in this setting. Further, when in a setting with symbolic toys, autistic children were more likely to spend their time focusing attention solely on objects (large effect). No interaction was found between play setting and total number of utterances spoken by autistic children. Conclusions and implications: This study confirmed the importance of continued research focused on understanding the relationship between children's play settings and their social engagement and language use. Although preliminary, findings support the idea that there is an interaction between preschool-aged autistic children's social engagement and their play settings. Further, our results suggest that there can be value in clinicians differentiating children's play settings (i.e., gross motor vs symbolic) when assessing and supporting social engagement capacities of young autistic children.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorders; communication and language; intervention/therapy; parent-child interaction therapy; pre-school children.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Pirateplots of descriptive data (group means, 95% confidence intervals) for engagement states and number of utterances across symbolic and gross motor play settings. Note. Gross = Gross Motor Setting; Sym = Symbolic Setting.

References

    1. Abbeduto L., Benson G., Short K., Dolish J. (1995). Effects of sampling context on the expressive language of children and adolescents with mental retardation. Mental Retardation, 33(5), 279. PMID: 7476250. - PubMed
    1. Adamson L. B. (1996). Communication development during infancy. Westview.
    1. Adamson L. B., Bakeman R., Deckner D. F., Nelson P. B. (2012). Rating parent-child interactions: Joint engagement, communication dynamic, and shared topics in autism, down syndrome, and typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2622–2635. 10.1007/s10803-012-1520-1 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Adamson L. B., Bakeman R., Russell C., Deckner D. (2000). Coding symbol infused joint engagement states. Technical report 9. Developmental Laboratory Department of Psychology Georgia State University.
    1. Adamson L. B., Chance S. (1998). Co-ordinating attention to people, objects, and symbols. In Wetherby A. M., Warren S. F., Reichle J. (Eds.), Transitions in prelinguistic communication: Preintentional to intentional and presymbolic to symbolic (pp. 15–37). Brookes.

LinkOut - more resources