Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Mar;28(2):339-366.
doi: 10.1111/infa.12515. Epub 2022 Nov 20.

Responding to joint attention as a developmental catalyst: Longitudinal associations with language and social responsiveness

Affiliations

Responding to joint attention as a developmental catalyst: Longitudinal associations with language and social responsiveness

Carolyn Lasch et al. Infancy. 2023 Mar.

Abstract

Joint attention (JA), infants' ability to engage in triadic attention with another person and a separate object or event, emerges in infancy. Responding to joint attention (RJA) develops earlier than initiating joint attention (IJA) and may benefit from a reconceptualization from a competence to a skill that varies in performance. Investigating associations between RJA performance and important skills of toddlerhood such as language, social responsiveness, and executive function (EF) in typically developing samples can better elucidate how RJA may serve as a developmental precursor to later dimensional skills, with implications for both typical and atypical development. Here, 210 (82% White) infants completed the Dimensional Joint Attention Assessment (DJAA), a naturalistic play-based assessment of RJA, at 8-15 months. At 16-38 months social responsiveness, verbal ability, and EF were assessed. Multilevel models showed that DJAA scores were associated with later verbal abilities and parent-reported social responsiveness. Exploratory analyses showed trend-level associations between RJA and EF. Results establish the content validity of the DJAA as a measure of RJA, and longitudinal associations with later verbal ability and social responsiveness. Future work should examine EF emergence and consolidation, and RJA and later EF associations.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

CML and JTE has no conflicts to declare. SMC is a Co‐founder and holds equity in Reflection Sciences, Inc., which has licensed the Minnesota Executive Function Scale (MEFS) from the University of Minnesota. These interests have been reviewed and managed by the University of Minnesota in accordance with its Conflict of Interest policies.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Ages at which participants completed various measures. (a) Each row represents a participant. Each data point is a visit, which may include multiple assessments (e.g., vrRSB and outcome MSEL collected at the same visit/age). (b) Age distributions for each measure. DJAA, Dimensional Joint Attention Assessment; MSEL, Mullen Scales of Early Learning; MEFS, Minnesota Executive Function Scale; vrRSB, Video‐Reference Rating of Reciprocal Social Behavior
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
A typical sequence of DJAA bids, all preceded by gaining an infant's visual attention. (a) First bid includes gaze and head turn. (b) Second bid includes gaze, head turn, and verbal cue. (c) Third cue includes gaze, head turn, and point. (d) Fourth bid includes gaze, head turn, point, and verbal cue. Sequence in administered from (a) to (d)
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Dimensional Joint Attention Assessment (DJAA) score at 8–15 months is associated with later verbal abilities as measured by the MSEL (Mullen Scales of Early Learning). Y‐axis values are derived from fitted MSEL scores in a multilevel model controlling for sex, age at outcome assessment, maternal graduate education, and verbal ability at DJAA assessment and are utilized to better illustrate the DJAA‐MSEL association
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Age‐conditioned Dimensional Joint Attention Assessment (DJAA) mean scores are associated with score on the Video‐Reference Rating of Reciprocal Social Behavior (vrRSB) at 16–31 months, after accounting for participant age, family income, and maternal graduate degree attainment
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Family income is increasingly associated with Minnesota Executive Function Scales (MEFS) total score from 24 to 38 months

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Akaike, H. (1973). Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In Petrov B. N. & Csaki F. (Eds.), Second international symposium on information theory (pp. 267–281).
    1. Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62(5), 875–890. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01577.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bates, D. , Mächler, M. , Bolker, B. , & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed‐effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 - DOI
    1. Bottema‐Beutel, K. (2016). Associations between joint attention and language in autism spectrum disorder and typical development: A systematic review and meta‐regression analysis. Autism Research, 9(10), 1021–1035. 10.1002/aur.1624 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brady, N. C. , Fleming, K. , Thiemann‐Bourque, K. , Olswang, L. , Dowden, P. , Saunders, M. D. , & Marquis, J. (2012). Development of the communication complexity scale. American Journal of Speech‐Language Pathology, 21(1), 16–28. 10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0099 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types