Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Aug 31:5:2396941520950979.
doi: 10.1177/2396941520950979. eCollection 2020 Jan-Dec.

Initiation and response of joint attention bids in autism spectrum disorder children depend on the visibility of the target

Affiliations

Initiation and response of joint attention bids in autism spectrum disorder children depend on the visibility of the target

Federica Cilia et al. Autism Dev Lang Impair. .

Abstract

Background and aims: Response to joint attention (RJA) and initiation of joint attention (IJA) are impaired in pre-schoolers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Children's response to joint attention may depend on the presence of the target in the child's field of vision or on the type of deictic index (head and eye orientation, pointing and verbalisations) used by the adult to initiate joint attention.

Methods: This study deals with 50 ASD children aged from 2 years 8 months to 11 years 7 months, with a communicative level comparable to children under 31 months of age, according to the French version of the Early Social Communication Scales (ECSP, by its French acronym). We compared the aforementioned ASD children with 50 typically developing (TD) children, aged 9 to 30 months who had no communication disorders according to the ECSP. During the ECSP test, we analysed joint attention behaviours on three posters present or absent from the children's visual field.

Results: We did not observe any difference in the number of IJAs between groups, but ASD children were less responsive than TD children were. Our results showed a developmental progression in the responses of children with ASD if several deictic cues were used by an adult simultaneously (looking and pointing, or looking and verbalising to indicate a target), whether the referent was present or absent from the child's visual field. In addition, we observed developmental progression when the referent was behind the child and the adult only used their gaze to refer to it.

Conclusion: Thus, we argue that the type of cues used affects ASD children's response behaviours during joint attention towards a referent that may be present or absent from their visual field.Implications: Regarding the social and the sensory difficulties of children with ASD, many therapeutic approaches focus their intervention methods on non-verbal communication skills and joint attention. This fundamental research makes it possible to identify the most appropriate type of deictic index for children with ASD with developmental delay, depending on the presence or absence of the referent in the child's visual field.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder; deictic behaviours; joint attention.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Photos extracted from the “poster session” of the ECSP test. Note. The child sits next to his mother. He initiates or responds to joint attention about posters placed to his left, right or behind him. The author of this article has written permission from adults as well as from the child's parents for the publication of these photograph.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Average number of initiations and responses to joint attention on a referent present or absent in the child's visual field for the group of children with autism (ASD) and for the group of typical children (TD). Note. Error bars show standard errors. *p < 0.05.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Developmental trajectories of response to the Looking index (top), Looking and verbalizations (middle), Looking and pointing cues (bottom) to a poster present (left) or absent (right) from the visual field for the group of children with autism (ASD) and for the group of typical children (TD).

References

    1. Adamson L. B., Bakeman R., Deckner D. F. (2004). The development of symbol-infused joint engagement. Child Development, 75, 1171–1187. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00732.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-5. American Psychiatric Publishing.
    1. Bakeman R., Adamson L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289. 10.2307/1129997 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baron-Cohen S. (1989). Joint-attention deficits in autism: Toward a cognitive analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185–189.
    1. Behne T., Liszkowski U., Carpenter M., Tomasello M. (2012). Twelve-month-olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 359–375. 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02043.x - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources