Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2022 Dec 1;12(1):20754.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-25016-5.

Inter-individual differences in baseline dynamic functional connectivity are linked to cognitive aftereffects of tDCS

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Inter-individual differences in baseline dynamic functional connectivity are linked to cognitive aftereffects of tDCS

Monika Pupíková et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has the potential to modulate cognitive training in healthy aging; however, results from various studies have been inconsistent. We hypothesized that inter-individual differences in baseline brain state may contribute to the varied results. We aimed to explore whether baseline resting-state dynamic functional connectivity (rs-dFC) and/or conventional resting-state static functional connectivity (rs-sFC) may be related to the magnitude of cognitive aftereffects of tDCS. To achieve this aim, we used data from our double-blind randomized sham-controlled cross-over tDCS trial in 25 healthy seniors in which bifrontal tDCS combined with cognitive training had induced significant behavioral aftereffects. We performed a backward regression analysis including rs-sFC/rs-dFC measures to explain the variability in the magnitude of tDCS-induced improvements in visual object-matching task (VOMT) accuracy. Rs-dFC analysis revealed four rs-dFC states. The occurrence rate of a rs-dFC state 4, characterized by a high correlation between the left fronto-parietal control network and the language network, was significantly associated with tDCS-induced VOMT accuracy changes. The rs-sFC measure was not significantly associated with the cognitive outcome. We show that flexibility of the brain state representing readiness for top-down control of object identification implicated in the studied task is linked to the tDCS-enhanced task accuracy.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Experimental design and methods. (a) The crossover design involved two sessions with real 2 mA stimulation/ sham tDCS with a concurrent working memory task. Prior to and after the stimulation, participants performed a visual object matching task (VOMT) and underwent resting-state fMRI. (b) Offline VOMT—subjects respond whether the two consecutive objects are the same or different by pressing a YES/NO button in two difficulty levels (conventional view of objects—lower difficulty level; unconventional view of objects—higher difficulty level). (c) Online WMT—subjects view a block of faces and scenes (2 + 2, randomized order) preceded by a specific command on how to react to a probe that follows each block. Subjects respond whether the probe is consistent/ inconsistent with the prior instruction by pressing a YES/NO button. Freely available face photographs from Chicago and Glasgow face databases were used as a face stimuli in the task.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Ten ICA components utilized for the rs-dFC analysis.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Four identified rs-dFC states (1–4 from the upper left to lower right). Each matrix depicts mutual correlations between each component identified using the ICA. Dark blue suggests a high negative correlation, and dark red suggests a high positive correlation. Note: DMN default-mode network, SMN sensorimotor network, DAN dorsal attentional network.

References

    1. Wang H, Abbas KM, Abbasifard M, Abbasi-Kangevari M, Abbastabar H, Abd-Allah F, et al. Global age-sex-specific fertility, mortality, healthy life expectancy (HALE), and population estimates in 204 countries and territories, 1950–2019: A comprehensive demographic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet. 2020;396:1160–1203. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30977-6. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Scott AJ, Ellison M, Sinclair DA. The economic value of targeting aging. Nat. Aging. 2021;1:616–623. doi: 10.1038/s43587-021-00080-0. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB. Structural brain changes in aging: Courses, causes and cognitive consequences. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;21:187–221. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2010.21.3.187. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel K. Normal cognitive aging. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2013;29:737–752. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009;60:173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types