Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Dec 14;12(24):3537.
doi: 10.3390/ani12243537.

Avian Alarm Calls Do Not Induce Anti-Predator Response in Three Anuran Species

Affiliations

Avian Alarm Calls Do Not Induce Anti-Predator Response in Three Anuran Species

Longhui Zhao et al. Animals (Basel). .

Abstract

Many species produce alarm calls in response to predators, and the anti-predator signals are often used by other species. Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls has been widely found in bird and mammal species. Other taxa, such as reptiles and amphibians, however, receive limited attention at present. Here, we selected three types of alarm calls of Japanese Tits (Parus minor) that were evoked by the Siberian Chipmunk (Eutamias sibiricus), Eurasian Sparrow Hawk (Accipiter nisus), and model snake (Elaphe spp.), respectively, and then carried out playback experiments to test whether three frog species changed their behaviors in response to the three treatments of Japanese Tit calls while the tit's territory song was used as a control. The results showed that Little Torrent Frogs (Amolops torrentis), Ornamented Pygmy Frogs (Microhyla fissipes) and Spot-legged Treefrogs (Polypedates megacephalus) did not jump off their positions in response to the same four acoustic signals. They also did not change their calling behaviors in response to the alarm calls of Japanese Tits. This study found no evidence that these anuran species can eavesdrop on heterospecific tits' alarm signals.

Keywords: anti-predator behavior; anuran; heterospecific eavesdropping; sound playback.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Spectrograms of four groups of Japanese Tit calls. (a) territory song; (b) alarm call evoked by the Siberian Chipmunk; (c) alarm call evoked by the Sparrow Hawk; (d) alarm call evoked by the snake.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Dall S.R.X., Giraldeau L.-A., Olsson O., McNamara J.M., Stephens D. Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2005;20:187–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Wagner R.H., Danchin E. A taxonomy of biological information. Oikos. 2010;119:203–209. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17315.x. - DOI
    1. Magrath R.D., Haff T.M., Fallow P.M., Radford A.N. Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: From mechanisms to consequences. Biol. Rev. 2015;90:560–586. doi: 10.1111/brv.12122. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bradbury J.W., Vehrencamp S.L. Principles of Animal Communication. 2nd ed. Sinauer; Sunderland, UK: 2011.
    1. Klump G.M., Shalter M.D. Acoustic behavior of birds and mammals in the predator context. 1. Factors affecting the structure of alarm signals. 2. The functional-significance and evolution of alarm signals. Z. Tierpsychol. 1984;66:189–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01365.x. - DOI

LinkOut - more resources