Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Jan 25;18(1):e0278067.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0278067. eCollection 2023.

Interoceptive differences in elite sprint and long-distance runners: A multidimensional investigation

Affiliations

Interoceptive differences in elite sprint and long-distance runners: A multidimensional investigation

Tom Seabury et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Interoception, the process of detecting and interpreting bodily sensations, may facilitate self-regulation and thereby play a crucial role in achieving elite performance in competitive sports. However, there is a lack of research conducted in world-class athletes. In the present research, two studies examined self-reported (interoceptive sensibility) and behavioural (interoceptive accuracy) interoception in elite (top 100 ranking) sprint and long-distance runners, and non-athletes. Study 1 used the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness Questionnaire. Sprinters reported having better regulation of attention to internal sensations, greater emotional awareness, better self-regulation, and reported a greater propensity to listen to their body for insight, than distance runners. Compared to non-athletes, sprinters and distance runners had more bodily trust, attention regulation, and self-regualtion. Additionally, elite athletes reported lower emotional awareness, self-regulation, and body listening. Study 2 examined cardioception using two tasks: The Heartbeat Counting Task, and The Heartbeat Detection Task. Elite and non-elite runners performed the tasks under two conditions; in silence, and whilst listening to pre-recorded crowd noise that simulated the live sounds of spectators during a sporting event. Sprinters and distance runners were able to maintain heartbeat detection accuracy when distracted, whereas non-athletes could not. Across both tasks, compared to non-athletes, sprinters and distance runners were more confident than non-athletes in their interoceptive percept. Additionally, elite athletes compared to non-elite athletes were less accurate when counting their heartbeat and were characterised by a higher interoceptive prediction error. Athletic populations have altered interoceptive abilities.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Positive feedback loop between physical activity and interoception.
Note. This model shows the reciprocal association between acute and chronic physical activity and interoceptive processes. Physical activity increasing the salience of interoceptive signals enhancing interoceptive accuracy and attention. Accurate internal models facilitate allostasis and minimise prediction error during subsequent exertion.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Nested design used in Study 1 and Study 2.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Raincloud plots showing interoceptive sensibility in sprinters, long-distance runners, and non-athletes.
Note. Data are adjusted means (after adjusting for anxiety, depression, gender, and age).
Fig 4
Fig 4. Raincloud plot showing heartbeat counting accuracy for elite and non-elite athletes.
Note. Data are means after adjusting for depression, anxiety, habitual exercise, and HR.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Raincloud plot showing heartbeat detection accuracy in athletes and non-athletes.
Note. Data are means after adjusting for depression, anxiety, habitual exercise, and HR. HBD–heartbeat detection.
Fig 6
Fig 6. Interoceptive processing in response to high and low intensity exercise.
Note. This model shows the role of exercise intensity in modulating the interaction between ‘top-down’ and ‘bottom-up’ interoceptive processes. During high intensity exercise interoceptive signals dominate perception such that it becomes more difficult to engage ‘top-down’ cognitive resources and predict future interoceptive states. Conversely, when exercise intensity is low, ‘top-down’ cognitive strategies are easier to maintain and future interoceptive states are easier to predict.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS, et al.. Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018;3(6):501–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Morgan WP, Pollock ML. Psychologic characterization of the elite distance runner. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1977;301:382–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1977.tb38215.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology. 2015;104:65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Murphy J, Brewer R, Plans D, Khalsa SS, Catmur C, Bird G. Testing the independence of self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2020;73(1):115–33. doi: 10.1177/1747021819879826 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mehling W. Differentiating attention styles and regulatory aspects of self-reported interoceptive sensibility. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2016;371(1708):20160013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0013 - DOI - PMC - PubMed