Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Jan:11:100481.
doi: 10.1016/j.jadr.2023.100481. Epub 2023 Jan 10.

Infant Negativity Moderates Trajectories of Maternal Emotion Across Pregnancy and the Peripartum Period

Affiliations

Infant Negativity Moderates Trajectories of Maternal Emotion Across Pregnancy and the Peripartum Period

Rebecca J Brooker et al. J Affect Disord Rep. 2023 Jan.

Abstract

Background: Although the effects of maternal behavior on the development of child emotion characteristics is relatively well-established, effects of infant characteristics on maternal emotion development is less well known. This gap in knowledge persists despite repeated calls for including child-to-mother effects in studies of emotion. We tested the theory-based postulate that infant temperamental negativity moderates longitudinal trajectories of mothers' perinatal symptoms of anxiety and depression.

Method: Participants were 92 pregnant community women who enrolled in a longitudinal study of maternal mental health; symptoms of anxiety and depression were assessed during the second and third trimesters of pregnancy and again at infant age 4 months. A multimethod assessment of infants' temperament-based negative reactivity was conducted at infant age 4 months.

Results: Maternal symptoms of anxiety showed smaller postnatal declines when levels of infant negativity were high. Negative reactivity, assessed via maternal report of infant behavior, was related to smaller postnatal declines in maternal anxiety, while infant negative reactivity, at the level of neuroendocrine function, was largely unrelated to longitudinal changes in maternal anxiety symptoms. Infant negativity was related to early levels, but largely unrelated to trajectories of maternal symptoms of depression.

Limitations: Limitations of this work include a relatively small and low-risk sample size, the inability to isolate environmental effects, and a nonexperimental design that precludes causal inference.

Conclusions: Findings suggest that levels of infant negativity are associated with differences in the degree of change in maternal anxiety symptoms across the perinatal period.

Keywords: Temperament; maternal anxiety; maternal depression; postpartum; pregnancy.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1:
Figure 1:
Trajectories of maternal anxiety and depression from the unconditional model. Trajectories are plotted on standardized scales to allow for simultaneous plots; scores are standardized within scale.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Latent growth model predicting trajectories of change in maternal anxiety symptoms from mother-rated infant negativity. **p < 0.01, +p < 0.10. Boxes reflect manifest variables; circles reflect latent variables. Model controlled for maternal symptoms of depression at each assessment. All loadings are shown on a standardized scale. Intercept was marginally correlated with linear (r = −0.23, p = 0.06) but not quadratic (r = 0.08, p = 0.57) slope. Linear and quadratic slopes were also correlated (r = −0.96, p < 0.01).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Latent growth model predicting trajectories of change in maternal anxiety symptoms from observed infant negative affect. **p < 0.01, +p < 0.10. Boxes reflect manifest variables; circles reflect latent variables. Model controlled for maternal symptoms of depression at each assessment. All loadings are shown on a standardized scale. Intercept was marginally correlated with linear (r = −0.21, p = 0.08) but not quadratic (r = 0.06, p = 0.67) slope. Linear and quadratic slopes were also correlated (r = −0.96, p < 0.01).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Latent growth model predicting trajectories of change in maternal anxiety symptoms from infant neuroendocrine reactivity. **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05. Boxes reflect manifest variables; circles reflect latent variables. Loadings for second trimester anxiety symptoms (shown in gray) were fixed. Intercept was correlated with linear (r = −0.24, p = 0.05) but not quadratic (r = 0.10, p = 0.47) slope. Linear and quadratic slopes were correlated (r = −0.96, p < 0.01).

Similar articles

References

    1. Andersson L, Sundström-Poromaa I, Wulff M, Åström M, & Bixo M (2006). Depression and anxiety during pregnancy and six months postpartum: A follow-up study. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 85(8), 937–944. 10.1080/00016340600697652 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Blair MM, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, & Davis EP (2011). Prenatal maternal anxiety and early childhood temperament. Stress, 14(6), 644–651. 10.3109/10253890.2011.594121 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brennan PA, Pargas R, Walker EF, Green P, Jeffrey Newport D, & Stowe Z (2008). Maternal depression and infant cortisol: Influences of timing, comorbidity and treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(10), 1099–1107. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01914.x - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Britton JR (2011). Infant temperament and maternal anxiety and depressed mood in the early postpartum period. Women & Health, 51(1), 55–71. 10.1080/03630242.2011.540741 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brooker RJ, Neiderhiser JM, Leve LD, Shaw DS, Scaramella LV, & Reiss D (2015). Negative affect in infancy elicits increases in parent anxiety symptoms over time. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1875. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01875 - DOI - PMC - PubMed