Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Jan 26:17:1094218.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1094218. eCollection 2023.

Impulsive choice in two different rat models of ADHD-Spontaneously hypertensive and Lphn3 knockout rats

Affiliations

Impulsive choice in two different rat models of ADHD-Spontaneously hypertensive and Lphn3 knockout rats

Monica S Carbajal et al. Front Neurosci. .

Abstract

Introduction: Impulsivity is a symptom of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and variants in the Lphn3 (Adgrl3) gene (OMIM 616417) have been linked to ADHD. This project utilized a delay-discounting (DD) task to examine the impact of Lphn3 deletion in rats on impulsive choice. "Positive control" measures were also collected in spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHRs), another animal model of ADHD.

Methods: For Experiment I, rats were given the option to press one lever for a delayed reward of 3 food pellets or the other lever for an immediate reward of 1 pellet. Impulsive choice was measured as the tendency to discount the larger, delayed reward. We hypothesized that impulsive choice would be greater in the SHR and Lphn3 knockout (KO) rats relative to their control strains - Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) and Lphn3 wildtype (WT) rats, respectively.

Results: The results did not completely support the hypothesis, as only the SHRs (but not the Lphn3 KO rats) demonstrated a decrease in the percent choice for the larger reward. Because subsequent trials did not begin until the end of the delay period regardless of which lever was selected, rats were required to wait for the next trial to start even if they picked the immediate lever. Experiment II examined whether the rate of reinforcement influenced impulsive choice by using a DD task that incorporated a 1 s inter-trial interval (ITI) immediately after delivery of either the immediate (1 pellet) or delayed (3 pellet) reinforcer. The results of Experiment II found no difference in the percent choice for the larger reward between Lphn3 KO and WT rats, demonstrating reinforcement rate did not influence impulsive choice in Lphn3 KO rats.

Discussion: Overall, there were impulsivity differences among the ADHD models, as SHRs exhibited deficits in impulsive choice, while the Lphn3 KO rats did not.

Keywords: Adgrl3; Lphn3 KO rat; attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); delay-discounting; externalizing behavior; latrophilin 3; response inhibition; spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR).

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Percent choice for the larger reward during acquisition (top) and maintenance responding (bottom) for the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) and WKY rats with equal trial lengths. The SHRs discounted the larger reward significantly more than the WKY rats at the longest two delays of acquisition and at all delays during maintenance, *p < 0.05.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
The slope of the discounting curve (top) was significantly more negative for the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) versus WKY rats, while the area under the discounting curve (bottom) was significantly less for the SHR versus WKY rats, *p < 0.05. The trial lengths were the same regardless of which lever was selected.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Percent choice for the larger reward during acquisition (top) and maintenance responding (bottom) for the Lphn3/ (knockout) and Lphn3+/+ (wildtype) rats with equal trial lengths. Genotype had no effect of on delay-discounting (DD) performance.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
The slope of the discounting curve (top) and area under the discounting curve (bottom) did not differ between the Lphn3/ (knockout) and Lphn3+/+ (wildtype) rats with equal trial lengths.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Percent choice for the larger reward during acquisition (top) and maintenance responding (bottom) for the Lphn3/ (knockout) and Lphn3+/+ (wildtype) rats with unequal trial lengths. Genotype had no effect of on delay-discounting (DD) performance.
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
The slope of the discounting curve (top) and area under the discounting curve (bottom) did not differ between the Lphn3/ (knockout) and Lphn3+/+ (wildtype) rats when trial lengths were not equal.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Acosta M. T., Castellanos F. X., Bolton K. L., Balog J. Z., Eagen P., Nee L., et al. (2008). Latent class subtyping of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and comorbid conditions. J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 47 797–807. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Acosta M. T., Swanson J., Stehli A., Molina B. S. MTA Team, Martinez A. F., et al. (2016). ADGRL3 (LPHN3) variants are associated with a refined phenotype of ADHD in the MTA study. Mol. Genet. Genomic Med. 4 540–547. 10.1002/mgg3.230 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Acosta M. T., Vélez J. I., Bustamante M. L., Balog J. Z., Arcos-Burgos M., Muenke M., et al. (2011). A two-locus genetic interaction between LPHN3 and 11q predicts ADHD severity and long-term outcome. Transl. Psychiatry 1:e17. 10.1038/tp.2011.14 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. American Psychological Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
    1. Aparicio C. F., Hennigan P. J., Mulligan L. J., Alonso-Alvarez B. (2019). Spontaneously hypertensive (SHR) rats choose more impulsively than Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats on a delay discounting task. Behav. Brain Res. 364 480–493. 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.040 - DOI - PubMed