Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Apr;10(2):023518.
doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.10.2.023518. Epub 2023 Mar 8.

Reproducibility of infant fNIRS studies: a meta-analytic approach

Affiliations

Reproducibility of infant fNIRS studies: a meta-analytic approach

Jessica Gemignani et al. Neurophotonics. 2023 Apr.

Erratum in

Abstract

Significance: Concerns about the reproducibility of experimental findings have recently emerged in many disciplines, from psychology to medicine and neuroscience. As NIRS is a relatively recent brain imaging technique, the question of reproducibility has not yet been systematically addressed.

Aim: The current study seeks to test the replicability of effects observed in NIRS experiments assessing young infants' rule-learning ability.

Approach: We conducted meta-analyses and mixed-effects modeling-based inferential statistics to determine whether effect sizes were replicable and comparable in a sample of 23 NIRS studies investigating infants' abilities to process repetition- and diversity-based regularities in linguistic and nonlinguistic auditory and visual sequences. Additionally, we tested whether effect sizes were modulated by different factors such as the age of participants or the laboratory. We obtained NIRS data from 12 published and 11 unpublished studies. The 23 studies involved a total of 487 infants, aged between 0 and 9 months, tested in four different countries (Canada, France, Italy, and USA).

Results: Our most important finding is that study and laboratory were never significant moderators of variation in effect sizes, indicating that results replicated reliably across the different studies and labs included in the sample. We observed small-to-moderate effect sizes, similar to effect sizes found with other neuroimaging and behavioral techniques in the developmental literature. In line with existing findings, effect sizes were modulated by the participants' age and differed across the different regularities tested, with repetition-based regularities giving rise to the strongest effects; in particular, the overall magnitude of this effect in the left temporal region was 0.27 when analyzing the entire dataset.

Conclusions: Meta-analysis is a useful tool for assessing replicability and cross-study variability. Here, we have shown that infant NIRS studies in the language domain replicate robustly across various NIRS machines, testing sites, and developmental populations.

Keywords: infant NIRS data; meta-analysis; replication crisis; rule learning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Optode arrangement employed in the studies included 8 or 10 sources and 8 detectors, forming a total of 20 or 24 channels. The region of interest that we focused on is the left temporal lobe, formed by channels 3 and 6; it is highlighted in red.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Schematic illustration of infant-level and study-level effect sizes calculations. Activation refers to the R versus 0, N versus 0, and R versus N contrasts computed as the average of the HRF along its time course. Magenta and cyan indicate repetition trials (HbO and HbR, respectively), and red and blue indicate nonrepetition trials (HbO and HbR, respectively).
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Organization of the dataset into the three sets of studies. The numbers overlaid on the bars indicate the number of participants in each category. Age is color coded (green is newborns, and violet is 6- to 9-month-olds).
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Forest plots of the meta-analytic effect sizes for brain activation in the left temporal area to repetition-based (R versus 0) and diversity-based (N versus 0) regularities, as well as for the difference in activation between them (R versus N) for HbO. Each study’s estimate is indicated by the corresponding square. Error bars indicate the 95% confidence interval. Diamonds show the summary estimates of each set of studies, with the center of the diamond corresponding to the estimate and the left and right edges indicating the confidence interval limits. Not all studies contributed to all comparisons, as described in Sec. 2.1.1 and Table 1. Corresponding forest plots obtained on HbR time traces are reported in the Supplementary Material (Fig. S1).
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Funnel plots of effect sizes against standard errors, for each of the three comparisons of interest, obtained on HbO (top panel) and HbR time traces (bottom panel); the white region shows the 95% confidence interval around the estimates, and the gray region shows the 95% to 99% interval; colors indicate the lab where the study was carried out, and shapes indicate the age group of each study.
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Distributions of individual effect sizes for the three comparisons of interest (R versus 0, N versus 0, and R versus N) for HbO (upper panel) and HbR (lower panel).
Fig. 7
Fig. 7
Meta-analytic effect sizes by age in sets 1 to 3. Gray bands indicate 95% confidence intervals, computed from the standard errors of each random-effects model. The top panel reports effect sizes obtained from HbO time traces, and the bottom panel shows the corresponding plots for HbR.
Fig. 8
Fig. 8
Grand average hemodynamic responses across studies in the left temporal area for the three comparisons of interest (R versus 0, N versus 0, and R versus N).

References

    1. Aslin R. N., “Questioning the questions that have been asked about the infant brain using near-infrared spectroscopy,” Cogn. Neuropsychol. 29(1–2), 7–33 (2012).COGNEP10.1080/02643294.2012.654773 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Gervain J., et al. , “Near-infrared spectroscopy: a report from the McDonnell infant methodology consortium,” Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 1(1), 22–46 (2011).10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.004 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Nosek B. A., Spies J. R., Motyl M., “Scientific utopia: II. Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability,” Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7(6), 615–631 (2012).10.1177/1745691612459058 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Munafò M. R., et al. , “A manifesto for reproducible science,” Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0021 (2017).10.1038/s41562-016-0021 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Open Science Collaboration, “Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science,” Science 349(6251), aac4716 (2015).SCIEAS10.1126/science.aac4716 - DOI - PubMed