Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Mar 13:12:e81318.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.81318.

Landmark-based spatial navigation across the human lifespan

Affiliations

Landmark-based spatial navigation across the human lifespan

Marcia Bécu et al. Elife. .

Abstract

Human spatial cognition has been mainly characterized in terms of egocentric (body-centered) and allocentric (world-centered) wayfinding bhavior. It was hypothesized that allocentric spatial coding, as a special high-level cognitive ability, develops later and deteriorates earlier than the egocentric one throughout lifetime. We challenged this hypothesis by testing the use of landmarks versus geometric cues in a cohort of 96 deeply phenotyped participants, who physically navigated an equiangular Y maze, surrounded by landmarks or an anisotropic one. The results show that an apparent allocentric deficit in children and aged navigators is caused specifically by difficulties in using landmarks for navigation while introducing a geometric polarization of space made these participants as efficient allocentric navigators as young adults. This finding suggests that allocentric behavior relies on two dissociable sensory processing systems that are differentially affected by human aging. Whereas landmark processing follows an inverted-U dependence on age, spatial geometry processing is conserved, highlighting its potential in improving navigation performance across the lifespan.

Keywords: geometry; human; human aging; landmark; neuroscience; spatial cognition; spatial navigation.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

MB, DS, SR, GT, AO, CA, JS, AA No competing interests declared

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Immersive Y-maze tasks to assess the relative influence of landmark and geometric spatial cues as a function of age.
(a) Top view of the Y-maze during the classical landmark condition (i.e., equiangular Y-maze; arm separation: 120°/120°/120°). Three distinct, distal landmarks (blue square, red circle, green star) cued the environment. (b) Top view of the Y-maze during the novel geometry condition (i.e., anisotropic geometric layout with no landmarks; arm separation: 50°/155°/155°). As depicted in the figure, the corridors in this condition were 54% longer than in the landmark condition to avoid the participants to see the end of the corridors from the starting locations. (c) Example of first-person perspective from the departure location during learning trials (i.e., position A in the maze) in the landmark and geometry condition (left and right, respectively). (d) Example of first-person perspective from the departure location during probe trials (i.e., position B in the maze) in the landmark and geometry condition (left and right, respectively). See Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for details on the real-world replica.
Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
Figure 1—figure supplement 1.. Real-world Y-maze implementation.
(a) Dimensions of the Y-maze in meters, as seen from above. Panels are 2.23 m high. A–C differentiate the three corridors (see protocol). Landmarks are 0.45 m2 pictures hanging from the ceiling at 2.82 m. They are positioned at 2.3 m from the maze center. (b, c) Customized Plexiglas panels were used to build the maze, which reflected the visible radiation while allowing infrared light to pass. Custom-made support frames held the IR-pass panels vertically.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Proportion of allocentric behavioral responses during probe trials in the three age groups.
Bar plots indicate the proportion of subjects who made either a majority (i.e., 3/3 or 2/3) or a minority (i.e., 1/3 or 0/3) of allocentric choices during the three probe trials. That is, green corresponds to allocentric responses, whereas yellow indicates egocentric behaviors. (a) In the landmark condition (n=42), children and older adults failed to solve the Y-maze task since they mostly adopted an egocentric behavior. By contrast, a significant majority of young adults were able to solve the task allocentrically. (b) In the geometry condition (n=37), the three age groups behaved similarly, with children and older adults mostly using an allocentric strategy as young adults. p-Values correspond to pairwise comparisons using Fisher’s exact test across the corresponding age groups and strategy preferences. The source data for this figure is available in the Figure 2—source data 1. This figure corresponds to the strategy preference observed in the virtual reality settings. The same data for the real-world replica can be found in Figure 2—figure supplement 1a.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.. Proportion of allocentric choices in the three probe trials.
(a) Proportion of subjects behaving allocentrically in the real-world Y-maze during the landmark condition (n = 17 participants: nine young and eight healthy older adults). (b–d) Proportion of subjects behaving allocentrically in the virtual Y-maze during landmark vs. geometry condition (n = 79 participants: 29 children, 22 young adults, and 28 healthy older adults). p-Values correspond to pairwise comparisons using Fisher’s exact test.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Spatial navigation performance during learning trials across the three age groups in the landmark (n=42, left) and geometry (n=37, right) conditions (a–f).
Colored lines represent median values for the three age groups. Box plots in (a) show the median (colored lines), the interquartile range (25th and 75th percentiles, length of the boxes), 1.5× interquartile range (whiskers) and outliers (dots). Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. p-Values in (a) shows uncorrected two-samples Mann–Whitney U tests.
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. Scatter plots of the navigation variables for the first four trials of the learning phase across the three age groups.
Color code: children (yellow), young (blue), and older (orange) adults.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.. Gaze-mediated exploratory behavior during spatial learning.
(a–c) Gaze dwell-time proportion for sky (a), floor (b), and wall (c) regions of the virtual space as a function of age (young adults n=22, older adults n=28) and experimental condition (landmark n=27, geometry n=23). We found a double dissociation between the time spent at visually exploring sky and floor regions in the landmark and geometry conditions (a and b, respectively). Neither age nor condition affected the gaze time proportion relative to the walls of the maze (c). Data were averaged across the four first trials. Box plots (a–c) show the median (colored lines), the interquartile range (25th and 75th percentiles, length of the boxes), 1.5× interquartile range (whiskers) and outliers (dots). (d) In the landmark condition, the spatial distribution of the visual focus of attention over the sky region showed that subjects gazed mostly at the landmark facing the departure point. Heatmaps data were pooled across age and the color bar normalization was computed for each group separately. (e) In the geometry condition, subjects mostly focused on the fork area of the Y-maze floor. (fg) Gaze dwell-time proportion in the sky region of the landmark condition(d) and the floor region of the geometry condition (e) as a function of learning trials, for young and older allocentric and egocentric subjects. No significant difference existed as either a function of age or strategy preference. Error bars show standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. Oculomotor behavior in the learning phase in young and older groups.
(a) Gaze dwell-time proportion for floor and wall regions as well as for the three landmark sectors of the sky region. (b) Spatial distribution of visual focus of attention over the sky region of the landmark condition (left) and over the floor region of the geometry condition (right), for young (top, n=22) and older (bottom, n=28) adults. Oculomotor data were averaged (in a) and pooled (in b) across the four learning trials. Heatmap color bar normalization in (b) was computed for each group separately.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.. Gaze dynamics in the probe trials of the landmark condition.
(a) Gaze dwell-time proportion relative to the sky region for young and older subjects. Independently from age, allocentric navigators explored significantly more the sky region in the probe trials (~40% of the trial) compared to the learning trials (~20% of the trial, see Figure 4f for a comparison). This result did not hold for older egocentric subjects, who spend ~20% of the trial gazing at the sky, irrespective of the learning or probe phases. Error bars show standard error of the mean. (b) For analysis purposes, the sky region was separated in landmark-centered sectors, as indicated by dashed lines. (c) Evolution of gaze dwell-times throughout the probe trials, including orientation and navigation periods, as a function of landmark sectors, age, and navigation strategy. The star sector corresponds to the landmark directly in front of the departure position in probe trials, while the circle sector corresponds to the landmark directly in front of the starting position in the learning trials. Allocentric young and older subjects focused on the star upon opening the eyes to reorient in space and plan their goal-oriented trajectories. During navigation, allocentric older adults switched their visual focus of attention onto the red landmark when being at the center of the maze. Egocentric older adults looked at the star during orientation as well as while navigating toward the center of the maze.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1.. Time spent gazing at the three landmarks during the first probe trial of the landmark condition (young adults, n=10 and older adults, n=17).
(a) During the orientation period, (b) during the navigation period, and (c) during the time spent in the central area of the environment. Box plots show the median (colored lines), the interquartile range (25th and 75th percentiles, length of the boxes), 1.5× interquartile range (whiskers) and outliers (circles). Young and older adults data. P-values from the Mann-Whitney U test.
Figure 6.
Figure 6.. Trajectories and gaze vector field representations in the first probe trials of the landmark condition in young and older adults.
Qualitative representations of goal-oriented trajectories color-coded with instantaneous speed (top rows), and gaze vectors (bottom rows) of six representative young (a) and older (b) allocentric navigators. Older adults tended to slow down at the center of the Y-maze where they eventually gazed at the red circle.
Figure 7.
Figure 7.. The presence of geometric cues eliminated the effect of age on navigation.
In the landmark condition, older allocentric adults took longer to reach the goal (a), were slower at reorienting in space (b), spent significantly more time in the central area of the maze (c), and their trajectories to the goal were longer compared to young allocentric adults (d). In comparison, there was no age difference in the geometry condition. Box plots in show the median (colored lines), the interquartile range (25th and 75th percentiles, length of the boxes), 1.5× interquartile range (whiskers) and outliers (circles). Stars indicate significant simple effect or Mann-Whitney test. These analysis were carried out in n=36 allocentric adults.
Figure 7—figure supplement 1.
Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Oculomotor behavior and navigation measures in the probe trials of the geometry condition.
(a) Gaze dwell-time proportion over the floor region for young allocentric subjects as well as older allocentric and egocentric navigators (there was no young egocentric subjects in the geometry condition). (b) Evolution of gaze dwell-time proportion for wall, floor, and sky regions across probe trials. (c–f) Time spent in central area, traveled distance, orientation duration, and escape latency during probe trials. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.
Figure 8.
Figure 8.. Predictive eye-motion statistics.
(a, b) Evolution of gaze altitude throughout probe trials. In both the landmark (a) and geometry (b) conditions, the gaze altitude during reorientation differed between allocentric and egocentric navigators. Eye level is denoted by 0. Shaded areas represent the between-subject standard error of the mean. (c) Distribution of the proportion of correct single-subject-single-trial predictions of the strategy used to solve the landmark condition of the Y-maze, based on the gaze altitude statistics during reorientation. The dashed vertical line indicates chance-level prediction, that is, the area to the left of the dashed line represents the probability (p-value) that less than half of the subjects in the validation set were correctly classified. (d) Prediction performance with respect to the experimental condition, that is, landmark vs. geometry, again on the basis of gaze altitude statistics during reorientation.
Figure 9.
Figure 9.. Visuo-cognitive multivariate analysis of age-related modulation of spatial behavior.
(a) Principal component analysis (PCA) across 19 measures of visual, attentional, mnemonic, and spatial reasoning capabilities (see Supplementary file 3 for test descriptions). Participants could be discriminated based on their age and, within the older population, their strategy preference. PCA was performed on 47 participants for whom we had the complete visuo-cognitive battery. (b–d) Scores of perspective taking, TMT mental flexibility, and contrast sensitivity. Error bars in (d) represent the standard error of the mean.
Figure 9—figure supplement 1.
Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Cognitive screening results for adult participants.
p-Values indicated to the left of the dashed vertical lines correspond to the comparison of the ‘Age effect’ (young vs. older adults, pooled across the two versions), whereas the other p-values concern the ‘Strategy effect’ (allocentric vs. egocentric within the older group of participants). See Supplementary file 3 for the description of all cognitive tests. We specify the n in each group since some subjects had one or more missing data among all the 19 measurements.
Figure 9—figure supplement 2.
Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Visual screening results for adult participants.
p-Values indicated to the left of the dashed vertical lines correspond to the comparison of the ‘Age effect’ (young vs. older adults, pooled across the two versions), whereas the other p-values concern the ‘Strategy effect’ (allocentric vs. egocentric within the older group of participants). See Supplementary file 3 for the description of all visual tests. We specify the n in each group since some subjects had one or more missing data among all the 19 measurements.
Figure 9—figure supplement 3.
Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Results from post-experiment self-reported visuo-spatial memory of landmarks.
(a) Recognition of the maze shape (among three possibilities). (b) Recall of the landmarks (among six possibilities). (c, d) Drawing of a top view map of the maze with the landmark array (c: example of a correct map; d: example of an incorrect one). (e) Quantitative performance of participants.
Author response image 1.
Author response image 1.

Comment in

References

    1. Adamo DE, Briceño EM, Sindone JA, Alexander NB, Moffat SD. Age differences in virtual environment and real world path integration. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2012;4:26. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00026. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Agathos CP, Bernardin D, Huchet D, Scherlen AC, Assaiante C, Isableu B. Sensorimotor and cognitive factors associated with the age-related increase of visual field dependence: a cross-sectional study. Age. 2015;37:9805. doi: 10.1007/s11357-015-9805-x. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Alberts BBGT, Selen LPJ, Medendorp WP. Age-Related reweighting of visual and vestibular cues for vertical perception. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2019;121:1279–1288. doi: 10.1152/jn.00481.2018. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bécu M, Sheynikhovich D, Tatur G, Agathos CP, Bologna LL, Sahel J-A, Arleo A. Age-Related preference for geometric spatial cues during real-world navigation. Nature Human Behaviour. 2020;4:88–99. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0718-z. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bohbot VD, McKenzie S, Konishi K, Fouquet C, Kurdi V, Schachar R, Boivin M, Robaey P. Virtual navigation strategies from childhood to senescence: evidence for changes across the life span. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2012;4:28. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00028. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources