Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 May 18;18(5):e0286067.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286067. eCollection 2023.

Causal implicatures from correlational statements

Affiliations

Causal implicatures from correlational statements

Samuel J Gershman et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Correlation does not imply causation, but this does not necessarily stop people from drawing causal inferences from correlational statements. We show that people do in fact infer causality from statements of association, under minimal conditions. In Study 1, participants interpreted statements of the form "X is associated with Y" to imply that Y causes X. In Studies 2 and 3, participants interpreted statements of the form "X is associated with an increased risk of Y" to imply that X causes Y. Thus, even the most orthodox correlational language can give rise to causal inferences.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Summary of results for Study 1 (left) and Study 2 (right).
Participants were given statements such as ‘Themaglin is associated with Pneuben’ (Study 1) or ‘Denoden is associated with an increased probability of Flembers’ (Study 2). A response of 1 indicates participants interpreted the statement to mean the first variable causes the second, 0 that they took it to mean the second variable causes the first, and 0.5 that they answered at random. The figure shows that without context, simple association is taken to imply the second variable causes the first. With minimal context, the association statement is taken to strongly imply the first variable caused the second. Error bars show standard error estimates for a proportion, p(1-p)/N, where p = M/N, M is the number ‘Yes’ responses, and N is the total number of responses.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Summary of results for Study 3.
Participants were given the same statements as in Study 2A, with the addition of a ‘Neither’ option to report that no causal direction was preferred. The y-axis now indicates the proportion of participants who chose each response. Error bars show standard error estimates for a proportion. The dotted line indicated expected random response levels.

References

    1. Grice HP. Logic and conversation. In: Speech Acts. Brill; 1975. p. 41–58.
    1. Clark HH. Using Language. Cambridge University Press; 1996.
    1. Goodman ND, Frank MC. Pragmatic language interpretation as probabilistic inference. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2016;20(11):818–829. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.08.005 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Garvey C, Caramazza A. Implicit causality in verbs. Linguistic Inquiry. 1974;5:459–464.
    1. Pickering MJ, Majid A. What are implicit causality and consequentiality? Language and Cognitive Processes. 2007;22:780–788. doi: 10.1080/01690960601119876 - DOI

Publication types