Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 May 25:14:1173486.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1173486. eCollection 2023.

The impact of bilingualism in within-language conflict resolution: an ERP study

Affiliations

The impact of bilingualism in within-language conflict resolution: an ERP study

Filip Andras et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

We compared Spanish (L1)-English (L2) bilinguals and Spanish monolinguals in a semantic judgment relationship task in L1 that produced within-language conflict due to the coactivation of the two meanings of a Spanish homophone (e.g., "hola" and "ola" meaning "hello" and "a wave" in English). In this task, participants indicated if pairs of words were related or not ("agua-hola," "water-hello"). Conflict arose because a word ("agua," "water") not related to the orthographic form of a homophone ("hola," "hello") was related to the alternative orthographic form ("ola," "wave"). Compared to a control condition with unrelated word pairs ("peluche-hola," "teddy-hello"), the behavioral results revealed greater behavioral interference in monolinguals compared to bilinguals. In addition, electrophysiological results revealed N400 differences between monolinguals and bilinguals. These results are discussed around the impact of bilingualism on conflict resolution.

Keywords: bilingualism; cognitive control; conflict resolution; inhibition; within-language conflict.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Mean response times per group and condition. The error bars display a 95% confidence interval.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Mean error rates per group and condition. The error bars display a 95% confidence interval.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
ERPs obtained in the homophone task. Event related potentials (ERPs) obtained in the related and unrelated condition when bilinguals and monolinguals performed the within-language conflict task. Vertical boxes indicate time windows in which the relatedness effect was significant in frontal, central and parietal scalp distributions.

Similar articles

References

    1. Alvarez R. P., Holcomb P. J., Grainger J. (2003). Accessing word meaning in two languages: An event-related brain potential study of beginning bilinguals. Brain Lang. 87 290–304. 10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00108-1 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Antoniou K. (2023). The ups and downs of bilingualism: A review of the literature on executive control using event-related potentials. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 1–40. 10.3758/s13423-023-02245-x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barulli D., Stern Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: Emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17 502–509. 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Beatty-Martínez A. L., Navarro-Torres C. A., Dussias P. E., Bajo M. T., Guzzardo Tamargo R. E., Kroll J. F. (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 46 1022–1047. 10.1037/xlm0000770 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bellegarda M., Macizo P. (2021). Cognitive control and bilingualism: The bilingual advantage through the lens of dimensional overlap. Front. Psychol. 12:614849. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.614849 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources