Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023:39:103452.
doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2023.103452. Epub 2023 Jun 12.

Mapping spoken language and cognitive deficits in post-stroke aphasia

Affiliations

Mapping spoken language and cognitive deficits in post-stroke aphasia

Haya Akkad et al. Neuroimage Clin. 2023.

Abstract

Aphasia is an acquired disorder caused by damage, most commonly due to stroke, to brain regions involved in speech and language. While language impairment is the defining symptom of aphasia, the co-occurrence of non-language cognitive deficits and their importance in predicting rehabilitation and recovery outcomes is well documented. However, people with aphasia (PWA) are rarely tested on higher-order cognitive functions, making it difficult for studies to associate these functions with a consistent lesion correlate. Broca's area is a particular brain region of interest that has long been implicated in speech and language production. Contrary to classic models of speech and language, cumulative evidence shows that Broca's area and surrounding regions in the left inferior frontal cortex (LIFC) are involved in, but not specific to, speech production. In this study we aimed to explore the brain-behaviour relationships between tests of cognitive skill and language abilities in thirty-six adults with long-term speech production deficits caused by post-stroke aphasia. Our findings suggest that non-linguistic cognitive functions, namely executive functions and verbal working memory, explain more of the behavioural variance in PWA than classical language models imply. Additionally, lesions to the LIFC, including Broca's area, were associated with non-linguistic executive (dys)function, suggesting that lesions to this area are associated with non-language-specific higher-order cognitive deficits in aphasia. Whether executive (dys)function - and its neural correlate in Broca's area - contributes directly to PWA's language production deficits or simply co-occurs with it, adding to communication difficulties, remains unclear. These findings support contemporary models of speech production that place language processing within the context of domain-general perception, action and conceptual knowledge. An understanding of the covariance between language and non-language deficits and their underlying neural correlates will inform better targeted aphasia treatment and outcomes.

Keywords: Aphasia; Cognition; Language production; Lesion-symptom mapping; Stroke.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of Competing Interest The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Structural correlates associated with each component from the combined PCA. Phonology: green; Executive Functions: magenta; Verbal Working Memory: yellow; Semantics: two distinct clusters in cyan and indigo. Clusters were obtained by applying a voxel-level threshold at p ≤ 0.001 and a family-wise error correction of p < 0.05 at cluster level. The lower right corner displays a rendered template brain (created in MRIcro-GL) showing the significant clusters projected to the left brain surface. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Lesion overlap map. A lesion overlap map for the 36 S anomic participants. Colour scale represents frequency of regional brain damage (hot-body scale with red indicating most frequently damaged brain regions i.e., >18 patients, while dark blue < 6 patients with damage to these regions). Results are shown overlaid on the MNI template brain, created in MRIcro-GL. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alexander M.P., Naeser M.A., Palumbo C. Brocas Area APHASIAS - APHASIA after lesions including the frontal operculum. Neurology. 1990;40:353–362. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.2.353. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Alyahya R.S.W., Halai A.D., Conroy P., Lambon Ralph M.A. A unified model of post-stroke language deficits including discourse production and their neural correlates. Brain. 2020;143:1541–1554. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa074. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ardila A., Bernal B., Rosselli M. How Localized are Language Brain Areas? A Review of Brodmann Areas Involvement in Oral Language. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2016;31:112–122. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acv081. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ashburner J., Friston K.J. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26:839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baldo J., Dronkers N., Wilkins D., Ludy C., Raskin P., Kim J. Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain Lang. 2005;92(3):240–250. - PubMed

Publication types