Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 May 30:14:1152904.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1152904. eCollection 2023.

Cross cultural verbal cues to deception: truth and lies in first and second language forensic interview contexts

Affiliations

Cross cultural verbal cues to deception: truth and lies in first and second language forensic interview contexts

Coral J Dando et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Introduction: The verbal deception literature is largely based upon North American and Western European monolingual English speaker interactions. This paper extends this literature by comparing the verbal behaviors of 88 south Asian bilinguals, conversing in either first (Hindi) or second (English) languages, and 48 British monolinguals conversing in English.

Methods: All participated in a live event following which they were interviewed having been incentivized to be either deceptive or truthful. Event details, complications, verifiable sources, and plausibility ratings were analyzed as a function of veracity, language and culture.

Results: Main effects revealed cross cultural similarities in both first and second language interviews whereby all liar's verbal responses were impoverished and rated as less plausible than truthtellers. However, a series of cross-cultural interactions emerged whereby bi-lingual South Asian truthtellers and liars interviewed in first and second languages exhibited varying patterns of verbal behaviors, differences that have the potential to trigger erroneous assessments in practice.

Discussion: Despite limitations, including concerns centered on the reductionary nature of deception research, our results highlight that while cultural context is important, impoverished, simple verbal accounts should trigger a 'red flag' for further attention irrespective of culture or interview language, since the cognitive load typically associated with formulating a deceptive account apparently emerges in a broadly similar manner.

Keywords: South Asian; cross cultural; detecting deception; first and second language; plausibility.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

    1. Akehurst L., Arnhold A., Figueiredo I., Turtle S., Leach A. M. (2018). Investigating deception in second language speakers: interviewee and assessor perspectives. Leg. Criminol. Psychol. 23, 230–251. doi: 10.1111/lcrp.12127 - DOI
    1. Albl-Mikasa M., Ehrensberger-Dow M., Hunziker Heeb A., Lehr C., Boos M., Kobi M., et al. . (2020). Cognitive load in relation to non-standard language input. Trans. Cogn. Behav. 3, 263–286. doi: 10.1075/tcb.00044.alb - DOI
    1. Al-Simadi F. A. (2000). Detection of deceptive behavior: a cross-cultural test. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 28, 455–461. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2000.28.5.455 - DOI
    1. Bull R., van der Burgh M., Dando C. (2019). “Verbal cues fostering perceptions of credibility and truth/lie detection” in The Palgrave handbook of deceptive communication. ed. Docan-Morgan T. (New York: Palgrave; )
    1. Buller D. B., Burgoon J. K. (1996). Interpersonal deception theory. Commun Theory 6, 203–242. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.1996.tb00127.x - DOI

LinkOut - more resources