Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Sep;54(9):3211-3224.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-023-06043-1. Epub 2023 Jul 6.

Gaze Following as an Early Diagnostic Marker of Autism in a New Word Learning Task in Toddlers

Affiliations

Gaze Following as an Early Diagnostic Marker of Autism in a New Word Learning Task in Toddlers

Raquel Camero et al. J Autism Dev Disord. 2024 Sep.

Abstract

The aim was to test the use of eye-tracking methodology for the early detection of ASD in a task of association between unfamiliar objects and pseudowords. Significant differences were found between ASD (n = 57) and TD (n = 57) Spanish speaking toddlers in the number and time of fixation. The TD children showed more and longer fixations on eyes and mouth while the ASD children attended almost exclusively to objects, making it difficult to integrate lexical and phonological information. Moreover, the TD toddlers looked at the mouth when the pseudoword was produced while the ASD toddlers did not. Gaze fixation on eyes and mouth during word learning recorded by eye-tracking may be used as a biomarker for the early diagnosis of ASD.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorders; Early diagnosis; Eye-tracking; Gaze following; Learning new words.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors report there are no competing interests to declare.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Sequence of a trial with identification of AOIs
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Mean percentage of fixation time to AOIs in ASD and TD
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Evolution of the number of gaze fixations by age group in the AIOs in ASD and TD children
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Evolution of the number of gaze fixations by age group in the AIOs in ASD and TD children

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bono, M. A., Daley, L. T., & Sigman, M. (2004). Relations among joint attention, amount of intervention and language gain in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 495–505. 10.1007/s10803-004-2545-x. 10.1007/s10803-004-2545-x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–543. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.x. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.x - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220. 10.1017/s030500090700829x. 10.1017/s030500090700829x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Camero, R., Martínez, V., & Gallego, C. (2021). Gaze following and pupil dilation as early diagnostic markers of autism in toddlers. Children, 8(2), 113. 10.3390/children8020113. 10.3390/children8020113 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Campbell, D. J., Shic, F., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Gaze response to dyadic bids at 2 years related to outcomes at 3 years in autism spectrum disorders: A subtyping analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 431–442. 10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9. 10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources