Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Jul 27;13(1):12190.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-39459-x.

Increased attention allocation to stimuli reflecting end-states of compulsive behaviors among obsessive compulsive individuals

Affiliations

Increased attention allocation to stimuli reflecting end-states of compulsive behaviors among obsessive compulsive individuals

Dana Basel et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Attentional research in OCD has focused solely on threat stimuli, assumed to provoke related obsessions and ensuing compulsions. OCD-related stimuli depicting the completion of compulsive acts ("end-states") have yet to be examined. Past research also neglected to explore the reliability of tasks used. Here, attention allocation to both stimuli types was examined. Participants with high (HOC) and low (LOC) levels of obsessive-compulsive symptoms freely viewed three blocks of 30 two-by-two picture matrices, each including two OCD-related (cleaning\checking\ordering) and two neutral pictures, presented for eight seconds, while their gaze was recorded. Participants completed two task versions - one with traditional threat stimuli and one with novel stimuli signaling compulsions end-states. Only the end-state version yielded significant results, showing that HOC participants, compared to LOC participants, spent significantly more time fixating on OCD-related stimuli. Results remained significant after controlling for anxiety, stress, and depression. Task reliability was high. OCD-related stimuli signaling end-states of compulsive behavior should be incorporated in attentional research in OCD.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
An example of a single matrix for the (a) traditional stimuli block; and (b) end-state stimuli block [Checking block (left), cleaning block (middle); ordering block (right)]. In each matrix each type of stimuli comprises a separate area of interest (AOI).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Total dwell time (in seconds) by Area of interest (AOI) and Group collapsed across blocks: (a) the traditional stimuli condition; and (b) the end-state stimuli condition. Error bars denote standard error of the mean. OCD obsessive–compulsive disorder, HOC high obsessive–compulsive tendencies, LOC low obsessive–compulsive tendencies.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ruscio AM, Stein DJ, Chiu WT, Kessler RC. The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Mol. Psychiatry. 2010;15:53–63. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.94. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Association, A. P. & Association, A. P. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (2013).
    1. Grabe HJ, et al. Lifetime-comorbidity of obsessive-compulsive disorder and subclinical obsessive-compulsive disorder in Northern Germany. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2001;251:130–135. doi: 10.1007/s004060170047. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mataix-Cols D, Vallejo J, Sanchez-Turet M. The cut-off point in sub-clinical obsessive-compulsive research. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 2000;28:225–233. doi: 10.1017/S1352465800003039. - DOI
    1. Welkowitz LA, Struening EL, Pittman J, Guardino M, Welkowitz J. Obsessive-compulsive disorder and comorbid anxiety problems in a national anxiety screening sample. J. Anxiety Disord. 2000;14:471–482. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(00)00034-7. - DOI - PubMed