Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Sep 1:14:1196481.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1196481. eCollection 2023.

Repressive and vigilant coping styles in stress and relaxation: evidence for physiological and subjective differences at baseline, but not for differential stress or relaxation responses

Affiliations

Repressive and vigilant coping styles in stress and relaxation: evidence for physiological and subjective differences at baseline, but not for differential stress or relaxation responses

Anna Exner et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Introduction: Previous research suggested differential stress reactivity depending on individuals' coping style, e.g., as classified by the model of coping modes. Specifically, stronger physiological reactivity and weaker subjective stress ratings were found for repressors than for sensitizers. However, it remains to be investigated (i) whether these findings, which are largely based on social stress induction protocols, also generalize to other stressors, (ii) whether repressors vs. sensitizers also exhibit differential stress recovery following the application of a relaxation method, and (iii) which stress reactivity and recovery patterns are seen for the two remaining coping styles, i.e., fluctuating, and non-defensive copers. The current study thus examines stress reactivity in physiology and subjective ratings to a non-social stressor and the subsequent ability to relax for the four coping groups of repressors, sensitizers, fluctuating, and non-defensive copers.

Methods: A total of 96 healthy participants took part in a stress induction (Mannheim Multicomponent Stress Test) and a subsequent relaxation intervention. Subjective ratings of stress and relaxation, heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV), and blood pressure were assessed during the experiment. HR and blood pressure are markers of the sympathetic stress response that can be regulated by relaxation, while HRV should increase with relaxation. To investigate long-term relaxation effects, subjective ratings were also assessed on the evening of testing.

Results: Despite successful stress induction, no differential responses (baseline to stress, stress to relaxation) were observed between the different coping groups on any of the measures. In contrast, a strong baseline effect was observed that persisted throughout the experiment: In general, fluctuating copers showed lower HR and higher HRV than non-defensive copers, whereas repressors reported lower subjective stress levels and higher levels of relaxation during all study phases. No differences in subjective ratings were observed in the evening of testing.

Conclusion: Contrary to previous research, no differential stress reactivity pattern was observed between coping groups, which could be due to the non-social type of stressor employed in this study. The novel finding of physiological baseline differences between fluctuating and non-defensive individuals is of interest and should be further investigated in other stressor types in future research.

Keywords: MMST; blood pressure; heart rate variability; relaxation; repressors; sensitizers; stress.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Subjective rating of perceived (A) stress and (B) relaxation after baseline, during stress induction and after relaxation for different coping styles. Error bars indicate the SEM; ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Figure 2
Figure 2
(A) Heart rate, (B) RMSSD, and (C) mean arterial blood pressure during baseline, stress and relaxation for different coping styles. Error bars indicate the SEM; **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

Similar articles

References

    1. Balzarotti S., Biassoni F., Colombo B., Ciceri M. R. (2017). Cardiac vagal control as a marker of emotion regulation in healthy adults: a review. Biol. Psychol. 130, 54–66. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.10.008, PMID: - DOI - PubMed
    1. Blum J., Rockstroh C., Göritz A. S. (2020). Development and pilot test of a virtual reality respiratory biofeedback approach. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 45, 153–163. doi: 10.1007/s10484-020-09468-x, PMID: - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Burgess D. J., Beach M. C., Saha S. (2017). Mindfulness practice: a promising approach to reducing the effects of clinician implicit bias on patients. Patient Educ. Couns. 100, 372–376. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2016.09.005, PMID: - DOI - PubMed
    1. Derakshan N., Eysenck M. W. (1997). Repression and repressors. Eur. Psychol. 2, 235–246. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.2.3.235 - DOI
    1. Derakshan N., Eysenck M. W., Myers L. B. (2007). Emotional information processing in repressors: the vigilance–avoidance theory. Cognit. Emot. 21, 1585–1614. doi: 10.1080/02699930701499857 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources