Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Mar-Apr;95(2):497-514.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.14008. Epub 2023 Sep 20.

Nameability supports rule-based category learning in children and adults

Affiliations

Nameability supports rule-based category learning in children and adults

Martin Zettersten et al. Child Dev. 2024 Mar-Apr.

Abstract

The present study tested the hypothesis that verbal labels support category induction by providing compact hypotheses. Ninety-seven 4- to 6-year-old children (M = 63.2 months; 46 female, 51 male; 77% White, 8% more than one race, 4% Asian, and 3% Black; tested 2018) and 90 adults (M = 20.1 years; 70 female, 20 male) in the Midwestern United States learned novel categories with features that were easy (e.g., "red") or difficult (e.g., "mauve") to name. Adults (d = 1.06) and-to a lesser extent-children (d = 0.57; final training block) learned categories composed of more nameable features better. Children's knowledge of difficult-to-name color words predicted their learning for categories with difficult-to-name features. Rule-based category learning may be supported by the emerging ability to form verbal hypotheses.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
(A) Experimental stimuli and category structure in the training phase. (B) Task design. Participants were asked which alien would like to eat the “snack” (color wheel) and were provided feedback after their selection. The progress bar depicts the number of “snacks” (trials) remaining.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Adults’ performance on the category learning task in the High Nameability condition (solid line) and Low Nameability condition (dashed line) during the two rounds of training. Horizontal dashed line indicates chance-level responding. Error bars represent ±1 SE of the within-subject corrected mean (Morey, 2008).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Children’s performance on the category learning task during the two rounds of training. Dashed line indicates chance level responding. Error bars represent ±1 SE of the within-subject corrected mean (Morey, 2008).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Relation between low nameability color word knowledge and categorization accuracy for adults and children in the High vs. Low Nameability condition.

Similar articles

References

    1. Anderson EM, Chang Y-J, Hespos S, & Gentner D (2018). Comparison within pairs promotes analogical abstraction in three-month-olds. Cognition, 176, 74–86. 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.008 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ashby FG, Alfonso-Reese LA, Turken AU, & Waldron EM (1998). A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review, 105, 442–481. 10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.442 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baayen RH, Davidson DJ, & Bates DM (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 - DOI
    1. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, & Tily HJ (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, & Walker SC (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 - DOI

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources