Eye-hand-coordination in man: a reaction time study
- PMID: 3801546
- DOI: 10.1007/BF00355600
Eye-hand-coordination in man: a reaction time study
Abstract
Reaction time measurements were used to test the possibility that an eye and a reach movement both aimed at the same visual target share a common final motor command. This hypothesis predicts highly correlated pairs of reaction times on a trial by trial basis. The experiments were based on the earlier observation that long saccadic reaction times (above 200 ms) with a large scatter (150-300 ms) are obtained if the central fixation point remained visible through out a trial (overlap paradigm), whereas extremely short saccadic reaction times (around 120 ms) with a small scatter (100-150 ms) occur if the fixation point was turned off some time (200 ms) before the target appeared (gap paradigm). In the overlap paradigm high correlations (coefficients up to 0.95) between saccadic and reach reaction times were obtained and the corresponding linear regression lines had a slope of almost one. In the gap paradigm, however, correlations were almost absent even though the mean reach reaction times were considerably decreased. These observations clearly contradict the "common command" hypothesis, but can be explained by the assumption that the execution of the reach movement depends on the completion of the preparation of the saccade but not vice versa ("one way synchronization" hypothesis).
Similar articles
-
Visually guided eye and hand movements in man.Brain Behav Evol. 1989;33(2-3):109-12. doi: 10.1159/000115910. Brain Behav Evol. 1989. PMID: 2758286
-
Reaction times of the eye and the hand of the monkey in a visual reach task.Neurosci Lett. 1985 Jul 4;58(1):127-32. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(85)90341-6. Neurosci Lett. 1985. PMID: 4047471
-
Further observations on the occurrence of express-saccades in the monkey.Exp Brain Res. 1986;63(3):487-94. doi: 10.1007/BF00237472. Exp Brain Res. 1986. PMID: 3758267
-
Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence.J Neurophysiol. 1996 Dec;76(6):3666-81. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.3666. J Neurophysiol. 1996. PMID: 8985865
-
Eye-hand-coordination: a model for computing reaction times in a visually guided reach task.Biol Cybern. 1986;55(4):263-73. doi: 10.1007/BF00355601. Biol Cybern. 1986. PMID: 3801547
Cited by
-
The parietal reach region is limb specific and not involved in eye-hand coordination.J Neurophysiol. 2014 Feb;111(3):520-32. doi: 10.1152/jn.00058.2013. Epub 2013 Nov 6. J Neurophysiol. 2014. PMID: 24198328 Free PMC article.
-
Eye-hand coordination during a double-step task: evidence for a common stochastic accumulator.J Neurophysiol. 2015 Sep;114(3):1438-54. doi: 10.1152/jn.00276.2015. Epub 2015 Jun 17. J Neurophysiol. 2015. PMID: 26084906 Free PMC article.
-
Prismatic displacement of vision induces transient changes in the timing of eye-hand coordination.Percept Psychophys. 1993 Sep;54(3):355-64. doi: 10.3758/bf03205270. Percept Psychophys. 1993. PMID: 8414894
-
Reaction time latencies of eye and hand movements in single- and dual-task conditions.Exp Brain Res. 1994;97(3):471-6. doi: 10.1007/BF00241541. Exp Brain Res. 1994. PMID: 8187858
-
Evidence of common and separate eye and hand accumulators underlying flexible eye-hand coordination.J Neurophysiol. 2017 Jan 1;117(1):348-364. doi: 10.1152/jn.00688.2016. Epub 2016 Oct 26. J Neurophysiol. 2017. PMID: 27784809 Free PMC article.
References
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Miscellaneous