Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Mar;45(4):e26550.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.26550. Epub 2023 Dec 5.

Somatosensory omissions reveal action-related predictive processing

Affiliations

Somatosensory omissions reveal action-related predictive processing

Tjerk T Dercksen et al. Hum Brain Mapp. 2024 Mar.

Abstract

The intricate relation between action and somatosensory perception has been studied extensively in the past decades. Generally, a forward model is thought to predict the somatosensory consequences of an action. These models propose that when an action is reliably coupled to a tactile stimulus, unexpected absence of the stimulus should elicit prediction error. Although such omission responses have been demonstrated in the auditory modality, it remains unknown whether this mechanism generalizes across modalities. This study therefore aimed to record action-induced somatosensory omission responses using EEG in humans. Self-paced button presses were coupled to somatosensory stimuli in 88% of trials, allowing a prediction, or in 50% of trials, not allowing a prediction. In the 88% condition, stimulus omission resulted in a neural response consisting of multiple components, as revealed by temporal principal component analysis. The oN1 response suggests similar sensory sources as stimulus-evoked activity, but an origin outside primary cortex. Subsequent oN2 and oP3 responses, as previously observed in the auditory domain, likely reflect modality-unspecific higher order processes. Together, findings straightforwardly demonstrate somatosensory predictions during action and provide evidence for a partially amodal mechanism of prediction error generation.

Keywords: EEG; efference copy; motor; predictive coding.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Schematic representation of the experimental design. Panel (a) shows the experimental set‐up: a participant sat in front of a screen with both arms on a table. With the right hand a button was pressed, possibly resulting in a stimulus on the left hand (indicated with green circles). Stimuli were applied by a puff of air traveling through air tubes and inflating a membrane on the left middle‐ and index‐finger. Panel (b) depicts the task over time, where participants pressed a button every 600–1200 ms. Panel (c) shows examples of the tactile effects of the button presses for all three conditions. In the 88%‐condition, there was an 88% chance of a button press resulting in a stimulus. In the 50%‐condition, the chance was 50%. In the motor condition, button presses never resulted in a tactile stimulus.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Uncorrected event‐related potentials (ERPs) for right‐lateral ROI (a: channels C6, CP6) and frontal ROI (b: channels Fz, F2). Plots show ERPs including 95% CIs for 88% omission, 50% omission, and motor‐control conditions.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Event‐related potential (ERP) amplitudes (colour map) and cluster statistics (transparency maps) for the difference between 88%‐ minus motor‐condition contrast in omission trials (panel a), 50%‐ minus motor‐condition contrast in omission trials (panel b), 88%‐ minus 50%‐condition contrast in omission trials (panel c), and 88%‐ minus 50%‐condition contrast in stimulus trials (panel d). Colour maps display the difference in ERP amplitude over time, broken down by electrode. Electrode numbers, broadly, begin at left frontal sites, ascending counter clockwise, first to posterior sites and then to right frontal sites. Statistically significant clusters (p < .05) are shown as opaque, while nonsignificant sampling points are shown as transparent.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Principal component analysis (PCA) omission components in chronological order (a–i). Plots show difference waves (condition minus motor) for reconstructed PCA (opaque) and the original event‐related potentials (ERPs) including 95% CIs (transparent) at highlighted (yellow) electrodes.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
First stimulus‐evoked components in chronological order (a–c). Plots show difference wave (condition minus motor) for reconstructed principal component analysis (PCA) (opaque) and the original event‐related potentials (ERPs) including 95% CIs (transparent) at highlighted (yellow) electrodes.
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
Comparison of topographies between stimulus‐evoked component 5 (88%‐condition and 50%‐condition) and omission component 1 (88%‐condition). Topographies show principal component analysis (PCA) activations at peak latency.

References

    1. Adams, R. A. , Shipp, S. , & Friston, K. J. (2013). Predictions not commands: Active inference in the motor system. Brain Structure and Function, 218(3), 611–643. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Allison, T. , McCarthy, G. , & Wood, C. C. (1992). The relationship between human long‐latency somatosensory evoked potentials recorded from the cortical surface and from the scalp. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 84(4), 301–314. - PubMed
    1. Andersen, L. M. , & Dalal, S. S. (2021). The cerebellar clock: Predicting and timing somatosensory touch. NeuroImage, 238, 118202. - PubMed
    1. Andersen, L. M. , & Lundqvist, D. (2019). Somatosensory responses to nothing: An MEG study of expectations during omission of tactile stimulations. NeuroImage, 184, 78–89. - PubMed
    1. Arikan, B. E. , Voudouris, D. , Voudouri‐Gertz, H. , Sommer, J. , & Fiehler, K. (2021). Reach‐relevant somatosensory signals modulate activity in the tactile suppression network. NeuroImage, 236, 118000. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources