Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Aug;31(4):1670-1679.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-023-02450-8. Epub 2024 Jan 16.

The impact of emotional valence on generalization gradients

Affiliations

The impact of emotional valence on generalization gradients

José A Alcalá et al. Psychon Bull Rev. 2024 Aug.

Abstract

Generalization enables individuals to respond to novel stimuli based on previous experiences. The degree to which organisms respond is determined by their physical resemblance to the original conditioned stimulus (CS+), with a stronger response elicited by more similar stimuli, resulting in similarity-based generalization gradients. Recent research showed that cognitive or conceptual dimensions also result in gradients similar to those observed with manipulations of physical dimensions. Such findings suggest that attributes beyond physical similarity play a role in shaping generalization gradients. However, despite its adaptive relevance for survival, there is no study exploring the effectiveness of affective dimensions in shaping generalization gradients. In two experiments (135 Spanish and 150 English participants, respectively), we used an online predictive learning task, in which different stimuli (words and Gabor patches) were paired with the presence - or absence - of a fictitious shock. After training, we assessed whether valence (i.e., hedonic experience) conveyed by words shape generalization gradients. In Experiment 1, the outcome expectancy decreased monotonically with variations in valence of Spanish words, mirroring the gradient obtained with the physical dimension (line orientation). In Experiment 2, conducted with English words, a similar gradient was observed when non-trained (i.e., generalization) words varied along the valence dimension, but not when words were of neutral valence. The consistency of these findings across two different languages strengthens the reliability and validity of the affective dimension as a determinant of generalization gradients. Furthermore, our data highlight the importance of considering the role of affective features in generalization responses, advancing the interplay between emotion, language, and learning.

Keywords: Emotion; Generalization gradients; Language; Predictive learning; Valence.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
(a) Gabor stimuli, (b) word stimuli. Note. Figure 1a represents Gabor stimuli in which the CS+ was 0° and the CS- was 90° (counterbalanced). GS refers to generalization stimuli, and the numbers symbolize the distance with the CS+. Figure 1b represents the four subsets of lists used in the experiment and the correspondence with Gabor stimuli. Bold words are the trained words. In these examples, words of valence around 2 were used as CS+ and words with valence around 8 were used as CS- (counterbalanced)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Generalization gradient. Note. Panel A represents the overall outcome expectancy for both stimuli dimensions. The CS+ was on the left part and the CS- on the right part of the figure. GS refers to generalization stimuli that were not presented during training. Numbers of GS refer to the proximity in terms of valence and orientation to the CS+ of each GS. GS1 was the closest to the CS+ and GS5 the furthest away. Error bars are the 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Panel B represents the violin plots of the slope considering the five GS. Black lines represent the medians and red lines the quartiles
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Expectancy test, Experiment 2. Note. Overall outcome expectancy for both stimuli dimensions. The CS+ is on the left part and the CS- on the right part of Fig. 3A. GS refers to generalization stimuli that were not presented during training. Number of GS refers to the proximity in terms of valence to the CS+ of each GS. GS1 was the closest to the CS+ and GS5 the furthest away. Error bars are the 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Panel B represents the violin plots of the slopes considering the five GS. Black lines represent the medians and red lines the quartiles

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alcalá, J. A., Miller, R. R., Kirkden, R. D., & Urcelay, G. P. (2023). Contiguity and overshadowing interactions in the rapid-streaming procedure. Learning & Behavior,51, 482–501. 10.3758/s13420-023-00582-4 10.3758/s13420-023-00582-4 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ahmed, O., & Lovibond, P. F. (2019). Rule-based processes in generalisation and peak shift in human fear conditioning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,72(2), 118–131. 10.1177/174702181876646110.1177/1747021818766461 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Anwyl-Irvine, A., Massonnié, J., Flitton, A., Kirkham, N., & Evershed, J. (2020). Gorilla in our MIDST: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods,52, 388–407. 10.1101/438242 10.1101/438242 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baron, A. (1973). Postdiscrimination gradients of human subjects on a tone continuum. Journal of Experimental Psychology,101(2), 337–342. 10.1037/h0035206 10.1037/h0035206 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Boyle, S., Roche, B., Dymond, S., & Hermans, D. (2016). Generalisation of fear and avoidance along a semantic continuum. Cognition and Emotion,30(2), 340–352. 10.1080/02699931.2014.1000831 10.1080/02699931.2014.1000831 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources