Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Jan 30;14(1):2469.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-51892-0.

Spatial hearing training in virtual reality with simulated asymmetric hearing loss

Affiliations

Spatial hearing training in virtual reality with simulated asymmetric hearing loss

Chiara Valzolgher et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Sound localization is essential to perceive the surrounding world and to interact with objects. This ability can be learned across time, and multisensory and motor cues play a crucial role in the learning process. A recent study demonstrated that when training localization skills, reaching to the sound source to determine its position reduced localization errors faster and to a greater extent as compared to just naming sources' positions, despite the fact that in both tasks, participants received the same feedback about the correct position of sound sources in case of wrong response. However, it remains to establish which features have made reaching to sound more effective as compared to naming. In the present study, we introduced a further condition in which the hand is the effector providing the response, but without it reaching toward the space occupied by the target source: the pointing condition. We tested three groups of participants (naming, pointing, and reaching groups) each while performing a sound localization task in normal and altered listening situations (i.e. mild-moderate unilateral hearing loss) simulated through auditory virtual reality technology. The experiment comprised four blocks: during the first and the last block, participants were tested in normal listening condition, while during the second and the third in altered listening condition. We measured their performance, their subjective judgments (e.g. effort), and their head-related behavior (through kinematic tracking). First, people's performance decreased when exposed to asymmetrical mild-moderate hearing impairment, more specifically on the ipsilateral side and for the pointing group. Second, we documented that all groups decreased their localization errors across altered listening blocks, but the extent of this reduction was higher for reaching and pointing as compared to the naming group. Crucially, the reaching group leads to a greater error reduction for the side where the listening alteration was applied. Furthermore, we documented that, across blocks, reaching and pointing groups increased the implementation of head motor behavior during the task (i.e., they increased approaching head movements toward the space of the sound) more than naming. Third, while performance in the unaltered blocks (first and last) was comparable, only the reaching group continued to exhibit a head behavior similar to those developed during the altered blocks (second and third), corroborating the previous observed relationship between the reaching to sounds task and head movements. In conclusion, this study further demonstrated the effectiveness of reaching to sounds as compared to pointing and naming in the learning processes. This effect could be related both to the process of implementing goal-directed motor actions and to the role of reaching actions in fostering the implementation of head-related motor strategies.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Experimental Timeline and Procedure. Schematic description of the experimental timeline. The first step was signing the consent and taking part in the audiometric examination. Then they performed a sound localization task. The task was composed of 4 consecutive blocks performed in different hearing conditions. Participants were divided in 3 groups, which were instructed to either naming, pointing or reaching the sound sources to localize them. Finally, participants were invited to answer some questions. We insert in the figure, three captures of the visual scene observable by the participants while doing the corresponding task and (to the right) an eye bird view representation of the speakers’ position relative to the head at the beginning of each trial.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Participants' performance. Absolute error (A) and signed error (B) across the four blocks (1: black; 2: red; 3: orange and 4: grey) and as a function of speaker position (x axis) and group (Naming: to the left, points; Pointing: center of the figure, triangles; Reaching: to the right, squares). Linear regression (solid line), with 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Participants' subjective judgments. Perceived effort (A), judgment about performance (B) and improvement (C) across the four blocks (1: black; 2: red; 3: orange and 4: grey) and as a function group (Naming: to the left, points; Pointing: center of the figure, triangles; Reaching: to the right, squares). Standard errors were reported.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Head-related behavior. Number of reversal (A), head-rotation extent (B) and approaching index (C) across the four blocks (1: black; 2: red; 3: orange and 4: grey) and as a function of speaker position (x axis) and group (Naming: to the left, points; Pointing: center of the figure, triangles; Reaching: to the right, squares). Linear regression (solid line). We did not include confidence intervals to increase the legibility of the graph.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Performance across trial repetition during altered hearing blocks. Absolute error (A) and signed error (B) as a function of trials during the altered hearing blocks (x axis), side (left or right) and group (Naming: continue line; Pointing: dashed line, triangles; Reaching: points line). Linear regression (solid line), with 95% confidence intervals.

References

    1. Middlebrooks JC. Sound localization. Handbook Clin. Neurol. 2015;129:99–116. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-62630-1.00006-8. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Pastore MT, et al. Effects of head movements on sound-source localization in single-sided deaf patients with their cochlear implant on versus off. Ear Hear. 2020 doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000882. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bahu H, Carpentier T, Noisternig M, Warusfel O. Comparison of different egocentric pointing methods for 3D sound localization experiments. Acta Acust. Acust. 2016;102(1):107–118. doi: 10.3813/AAA.918928. - DOI
    1. Mendonça C. A review on auditory space adaptations to altered head-related cues. Front. Neurosci. 2014;8:219. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00219. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Carlile S, Balachandar K, Kelly H. Accommodating to new ears: The effects of sensory and sensory-motor feedback. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014;135(4):2002–2011. doi: 10.1121/1.4868369. - DOI - PubMed