Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2024 Aug;24(4):617-630.
doi: 10.3758/s13415-024-01168-x. Epub 2024 Feb 21.

The neurobiology of aesthetic chills: How bodily sensations shape emotional experiences

Affiliations
Review

The neurobiology of aesthetic chills: How bodily sensations shape emotional experiences

Felix Schoeller et al. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2024 Aug.

Abstract

The phenomenon of aesthetic chills-shivers and goosebumps associated with either rewarding or threatening stimuli-offers a unique window into the brain basis of conscious reward because of their universal nature and simultaneous subjective and physical counterparts. Elucidating the neural mechanisms underlying aesthetic chills can reveal fundamental insights about emotion, consciousness, and the embodied mind. What is the precise timing and mechanism of bodily feedback in emotional experience? How are conscious feelings and motivations generated from interoceptive predictions? What is the role of uncertainty and precision signaling in shaping emotions? How does the brain distinguish and balance processing of rewards versus threats? We review neuroimaging evidence and highlight key questions for understanding how bodily sensations shape conscious feelings. This research stands to advance models of brain-body interactions shaping affect and may lead to novel nonpharmacological interventions for disorders of motivation and pleasure.

Keywords: Arousal; Chills; Dopamine; Emotional; Film; Learning; Music; Precision; Reward; Valence.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

In the past years, FS founded and received compensation from BeSound SAS and Nested Minds LTD. Over the past 3 years, Dr. Pizzagalli has received consulting fees from Albright Stonebridge Group, Boehringer Ingelheim, Compass Pathways, Engrail Therapeutics, Neumora Therapeutics (formerly BlackThorn Therapeutics), Neurocrine Biosciences, Neuroscience Software, Otsuka, Sunovion, and Takeda; he has received honoraria from the Psychonomic Society and American Psychological Association (for editorial work) and from Alkermes; he has received research funding from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, the Dana Foundation, Millennium Pharmaceuticals, Wellcome Leap MCPsych, and NIMH; he has received stock options from Compass Pathways, Engrail Therapeutics, Neumora Therapeutics, and Neuroscience Software; he has a financial interest in Neumora Therapeutics, which has licensed the copyright to the probabilistic reward task through Harvard University. Dr. Pizzagalli’s interests were reviewed and are managed by McLean Hospital and Partners HealthCare in accordance with their conflict of interest policies. No funding from these entities was used to support the current work, and all views expressed are solely those of the authors. All other authors declare they have no competing interests. All other authors have no conflicts of interest or relevant disclosures.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Neural mechanisms of chills and reward. Green areas indicate increased activation during the chills response. Red areas are deactivated. Key regions involved include the ventral tegmental area (VTA) projecting to the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens) and the hippocampus. The amygdala, orbital, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex show deactivation during chills (Blood & Zatorre, 2001). The VTA, nucleus accumbens, and striatum are part of the brain's reward system, associated with pleasure, reward, and compulsive behavior. The right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) plays a role in sensory processing, reward, and expected outcomes. Note that electroencephalographic recording of chills showed theta activity with activation in the orbitofrontal cortex, in correlation with emotional ratings (Chabin et al., 2020)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Three phases of pleasure and chills as peak consummatory pleasure. The "Wanting" phase represents the initial anticipation and desire for rewards. The "Liking" phase corresponds to the peak of consummatory pleasure, characterized by the experience of aesthetic chills (AC). Following the chills, the “Learning” phase begins, involving the encoding of crucial information about the film. This phase is associated with the consolidation of the CS meaning. In other words, AC marks the onset of the satiation process, where the viewer's curiosity is temporarily satisfied
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Learning (blue) and its rate of change (red), as the fit between ascending signals and available models. Learning is represented by the equation: L = ∏n [∑m l(X(n) | M(m))], where l denotes the conditional similarity of data given conditional models (Schoeller et al., 2018a, b). This sinusoidal representation is a gross oversimplification for the sake of readability. The blue area at the peak of the curve defines conscious aesthetic emotions (when the rate of change tends toward zero), and the squared area describes a descending learning slope corresponding to a negative derivative. The graph highlights the interplay between learning and emotions, emphasizing how insights (e.g., positive chills) and traumas (e.g., negative chills) shape an individual’s overall mood and well-being. Adapted from Schoeller & Perlovsky,
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
A and B. Individuals who experience aesthetic chills display higher psychological insight (PIS) and emotional breakthrough (EBI) compared with control (Schoeller et al., 2023c; 2023d). C and D. Chills intensity is positively correlated with emotional breakthrough (EBI) and emotional awareness (MAIA). E. AC was associated with a significant change in reward bias (PRT) pre- and post-exposure to stimuli in subjects compared to control (Jain et al., 2023b). F. Experience of aesthetic chills was reliably associated with patterns of ego dissolution, connectedness, and moral elevation (Christov-Moore et al., 2023)
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Circumplex model depicts participants’ self-reported arousal and valence before (empty dots) and after chills exposure (full dots). Participants with high anhedonia (HA) are shown in red, low anhedonia (LA) in orange, and healthy controls in blue. Solid lines indicate participants who experienced chills, while dotted lines show those who did not. Empty dots represent pre-exposure ratings, while filled dots show post-exposure ratings. HA participants who experienced chills shifted toward LA participants without chills and control pre-exposure levels. This suggests chills exposure increased arousal and valence in high anhedonia to approach levels seen in low anhedonia and controls

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adams, R. A., Stephan, K. E., Brown, H. R., Frith, C. D., Friston, K. J. (2013). The computational anatomy of psychosis. Front Psychiatry, 4, 47. 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00047 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Adcock, R. A., Thangavel, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Knutson, B., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2006). Reward-motivated learning: mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron, 50(3), 507–517. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.036 - PubMed
    1. Benedek M, Kaernbach C. Physiological correlates and emotional specificity of human piloerection. Biological Psychology. 2011;86(3):320–329. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.12.012. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191(3):391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Berridge KC. From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;35(7):1124–1143. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x. - DOI - PMC - PubMed