Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Feb 24;9(1):11.
doi: 10.1038/s41539-024-00223-z.

A history of avoidance does not impact extinction learning in male rats

Affiliations

A history of avoidance does not impact extinction learning in male rats

Alba López-Moraga et al. NPJ Sci Learn. .

Abstract

Pervasive avoidance is one of the central symptoms of all anxiety-related disorders. In treatment, avoidance behaviors are typically discouraged because they are assumed to maintain anxiety. Yet, it is not clear if engaging in avoidance is always detrimental. In this study, we used a platform-mediated avoidance task to investigate the influence of avoidance history on extinction learning in male rats. Our results show that having the opportunity to avoid during fear acquisition training does not significantly influence the extinction of auditory-cued fear in rats subjected to this platform-mediated avoidance procedure, which constitutes a realistic approach/avoidance conflict. This holds true irrespective of whether or not avoidance was possible during the extinction phase. This suggests that imposing a realistic cost on avoidance behavior prevents the adverse effects that avoidance has been claimed to have on extinction. However, avoidance does not appear to have clear positive effects on extinction learning nor on retention either.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1. Graphic representation of Experiments 1–3.
a Graphic representation of the different phases of Experiment 1. b Graphic representation of the different phases of Experiment 2. c Graphic representation of the different phases of Experiment 3.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2. Avoidance training results for Experiments 1–3.
The box plots represent the average of the first 3 CSs on each day. The bold lines in the trial-by-trial plots represent the mean and the surrounding shaded area the standard error of the mean. Results are expressed in % of time during CS presentations. a Graphic representation of the avoidance training sessions in the three experiments. b In the Avoider group, avoidance increased significantly from day 1 to 2 (V = 1, p < 0.001, r = 0.861). Across both groups, freezing increased significantly from day 1 to 2 (F(1, 22) = 59.80, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.731). c In the Avoider group, avoidance increased significantly from day 1 to 2 (V = 1, p < 0.001, r = 0.861). Across both groups, freezing increased significantly from day 1 to 2 (Q(1, 10.70) = 23.33, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.477). d In the Avoider group, avoidance increased significantly from day 1 to 2 (t(11) = −2.74, p = 0.019, d = −0.791). Freezing differed significantly between Avoiders and Yoked animals (F(1, 22) = 6.66, p = 0.017, ηp2 = 0.232). *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3. Extinction results for Experiments 1–3.
The box plots represent the average of the first 3 CSs on each day. The bold lines in the trial-by-trial plots represent the mean and the surrounding shaded area the standard error of the mean. Results are expressed in % of time during CS presentations. a Graphic representation of the extinction training sessions. b Across both groups, freezing decreased significantly over the extinction sessions (Q(3, 11.33) = 87.44, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.764). A further investigation of the interaction between group and extinction session (Q(3, 10.61) = 7.14, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.13) revealed that on day 1 the Avoider group suppressed lever pressing more than the Yoked group (V = 135, p < 0.001), whereas on day 2 the Yoked group shower more suppression of lever pressing than the Avoider group (V = 32.5, p = 0.024). c In the Avoider group, avoidance decreased significantly over the extinction sessions (χ2(3) = 28, p < 0.001, W = 0.779). Freezing decreased significantly across both groups (Q(3, 9.6) = 30.56, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.694), as did lever pressing (Q(3, 10.98) = 258.58, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.77). d In the Avoider group, avoidance decreased significantly from day 1 to 2 (t(10) = 5.9, p < 0.001, d = 1.78). An interaction between group and test day (Q(1, 11.76) = 8.55, p = 0.013) revealed that freezing was significantly decreased in the Avoider group (V = 52, p = 0.014, r = 0.476), but not the Yoked group. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4. Reinstatement test results for Experiment 2 and 3.
The box plots represent the average of the first 3 CSs on each day. The bold lines in the trial-by-trial plots represent the mean and the surrounding shaded area the standard error of the mean. Results are expressed in % of time during CS presentations. a Graphical representation of reinstatement and the reinstatement test sessions in Experiment 2 and 3. b Suppression of lever pressing differed significantly between the Avoider and the Yoked group (Q(1, 13.94) = 5.58, p = 0.042, ηp2 = 0.175). c Freezing decreased significantly between last day of extinction training and reinstatement test (Q(1, 11.33) = 6.72, p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.286), as did suppression of lever pressing (F(1, 21) = 5.31, p = 0.031, ηp2 = 0.202). *p < 0.05.
Fig. 5
Fig. 5. Spontaneous recovery test results for Experiment 3.
The box plots represent the average of the first 3 CSs on each day. The bold lines in the trial-by-trial plots represent the mean and the surrounding shaded area the standard error of the mean. Results are expressed in % of time during CS presentations. a Graphical representation of the last extinction session and the spontaneous recovery test session. b Freezing increased significantly from the last extinction session to the spontaneous recovery test session (Q(1, 9.72) = 25.61, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.600). Similarly, suppression of lever pressing increased significantly between the last extinction session and the spontaneous recovery test session (Q(1, 10.65) = 10.99, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.463). **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th edn (American Psychiatric Association, 2014).
    1. Sharpe L, et al. Safety behaviours or safety precautions? The role of subtle avoidance in anxiety disorders in the context of chronic physical illness. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2022;92:102126. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102126. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Arnaudova I, Kindt M, Fanselow M, Beckers T. Pathways towards the proliferation of avoidance in anxiety and implications for treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 2017;96:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.04.004. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Lovibond PF, Mitchell CJ, Minard E, Brady A, Menzies RG. Safety behaviours preserve threat beliefs: protection from extinction of human fear conditioning by an avoidance response. Behav. Res. Ther. 2009;47:716–720. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.04.013. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Solomon RL, Kamin LJ, Wynne LC. Traumatic avoidance learning: the outcomes of several extinction procedures with dogs. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1953;48:291–302. doi: 10.1037/h0058943. - DOI - PubMed