Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Feb 7;70(1):87-97.
doi: 10.1093/cz/zoad001. eCollection 2024 Feb.

Invasive ant learning is not affected by seven potential neuroactive chemicals

Affiliations

Invasive ant learning is not affected by seven potential neuroactive chemicals

Henrique Galante et al. Curr Zool. .

Abstract

Argentine ants Linepithema humile are one of the most damaging invasive alien species worldwide. Enhancing or disrupting cognitive abilities, such as learning, has the potential to improve management efforts, for example by increasing preference for a bait, or improving ants' ability to learn its characteristics or location. Nectar-feeding insects are often the victims of psychoactive manipulation, with plants lacing their nectar with secondary metabolites such as alkaloids and non-protein amino acids which often alter learning, foraging, or recruitment. However, the effect of neuroactive chemicals has seldomly been explored in ants. Here, we test the effects of seven potential neuroactive chemicals-two alkaloids: caffeine and nicotine; two biogenic amines: dopamine and octopamine, and three nonprotein amino acids: β-alanine, GABA and taurine-on the cognitive abilities of invasive L. humile using bifurcation mazes. Our results confirm that these ants are strong associative learners, requiring as little as one experience to develop an association. However, we show no short-term effect of any of the chemicals tested on spatial learning, and in addition no effect of caffeine on short-term olfactory learning. This lack of effect is surprising, given the extensive reports of the tested chemicals affecting learning and foraging in bees. This mismatch could be due to the heavy bias towards bees in the literature, a positive result publication bias, or differences in methodology.

Keywords: associative learning; caffeine; memory; neuroactive chemicals.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest related to this work.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Ants learn to associate a sucrose reward with an arm of a Y-maze over consecutive visits across experiments and treatments. Circles represent the proportion of ants choosing the rewarded side of the maze as their final choice and whiskers the respective 95% unconditional confidence intervals for each treatment. Estimates for each experiment were obtained from model-averaging with shrinkage and estimated marginal means were averaged over the side of the maze in which the reward was located and the elapsed time since the end of the first visit. This was done as the confidence intervals for the model averaged odds ratios for both reward side and elapsed time crossed 1, suggesting small differences between the categorical levels of these variables. The exception to this, with an odds ratio of 0.2 [0.07, 0.89], being the octopamine experiment which showed a relatively large side bias towards the left (L = 93%, R = 77%). However, since even ants with the reward on the right were able to learn the association, we average both sides. If the confidence intervals of each estimate include 50% (red dashed horizontal line), ants are considered to choose an arm of the Y-maze at random and therefore likely did not learn. Significance levels were adjusted using Bonferroni correction for multiple testing.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Effect of seven potential neuroactive chemicals on the olfactory and spatial associative learning of L. humile. Circles represent the estimates obtained from model-averaging with shrinkage and whiskers the 95% unconditional confidence intervals. The odds ratio compares the odds (probability of an event occurring divided by the probability of the event not occurring) of the ants choosing the rewarded side of the Y-maze under the influence of each neuroactive chemical against those of the corresponding control treatment. Odds ratios of 1 (red dashed vertical line) indicate no difference between the treatment and its control, whilst odds ratios > 1 or < 1 indicate that ants are more or less likely, respectively, to choose the rewarded side of the Y-maze under the influence of the neuroactive chemical when compared to the control. If the 95% confidence intervals include an odds ratio of 1 there is no significant difference between treatment and control. R2 refers to the goodness of fit of the model which explains the most variance in the data for each set of candidate models.

References

    1. Agarwal M, Guzmán MG, Morales-Matos C, Díaz RADV, Abramson CIet al. , 2011. Dopamine and octopamine influence avoidance learning of honey bees in a place preference assay. PLoS One 6(9):e25371. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson DR, 2008. Model Based Inference in the Life Sciences: A Primer on Evidence. New York: Springer.
    1. Angulo E, Hoffmann BD, Ballesteros-Mejia L, Taheri A, Balzani Pet al. , 2022. Economic costs of invasive alien ants worldwide. Biol Invasions 24(1):2041–2060.
    1. Arenas A, Roces F, 2018. Appetitive and aversive learning of plants odors inside different nest compartments by foraging leaf-cutting ants. J Insect Physiol 109(1):85–92. - PubMed
    1. Arnold SEJ, Dudenhöffer J, Fountain MT, James KL, Hall DRet al. , 2021. Bumble bees show an induced preference for flowers when primed with caffeinated nectar and a target floral odor. Curr Biol 31(18):4127–4131. - PubMed