Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Jul 1:198:107385.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2024.107385. Epub 2024 Apr 29.

Hedonic hunger, ultra-processed food consumption, and the moderating effects of impulsivity in pregnant individuals with body mass index ≥ 25

Affiliations

Hedonic hunger, ultra-processed food consumption, and the moderating effects of impulsivity in pregnant individuals with body mass index ≥ 25

Riley J Jouppi et al. Appetite. .

Abstract

Evidence suggests higher hedonic hunger (preoccupation with/desire to consume food for pleasure) is associated with greater ultra-processed food (UPF) consumption in non-pregnant individuals with higher, but not lower, self-report impulsivity or delay discounting. The current study tested the association between hedonic hunger and UPF consumption, and the moderating effects of self-report impulsivity and delay discounting, during pregnancy. Individuals (N = 220) with body mass index (BMI)≥25 completed the Power of Food Scale, 24-h dietary recalls, and Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Version 11 in early-mid pregnancy. A subset enrolled in an ancillary study (n = 143) completed a Delay Discounting Task. Linear regression and moderation models covaried for age, gestational age, pre-pregnancy BMI, and socioeconomic status. The association between hedonic hunger and UPF consumption was nonsignificant (p = 0.47). Self-report impulsivity was not a significant moderator (p = 0.11), but delay discounting was (p = 0.01). Simple slopes analysis revealed a one-unit increase in hedonic hunger was associated with 7% lower UPF intake among participants with lower (M+1SD) delay discounting (p = 0.01) and 1% higher UPF intake among those with higher (M-1SD) delay discounting (p = 0.57). Findings contrast those from research with non-pregnant samples and indicate lower delay discounting may serve as a protective factor, associated with reduced UPF consumption at higher levels of hedonic hunger, during pregnancy.

Keywords: Hedonic hunger; Impulsivity; Obesity; Overweight; Pregnancy; Ultra-processed food.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of competing interest The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Moderating effect of delay discounting on the association between hedonic hunger and ultra-processed food (UPF) consumption
Note. * indicates statistical significance (p < .05). Hedonic hunger (PFS total score) and delay discounting (AUCDDT) were mean-centered. “Higher” (n = 37) and “lower” (n = 35) delay discounting were determined by respectively subtracting and adding 1 standard deviation (0.35) from and to the adjusted mean (0.00).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Moderating effect of self-report impulsivity on the association between appetitive responses at the “food available” level and ultra-processed food (UPF) consumption
Note. * indicates statistical significance (p < .05). Appetitive responses at the “food available” level (PFS Availability subscale score) and self-report impulsivity (BIS-11 total score) were mean-centered. “Higher” (n = 35) and “lower” (n = 33) self-report impulsivity were determined by respectively adding and subtracting 1 standard deviation (10.71) to and from the adjusted mean (0.00).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Moderating effect of delay discounting on the association between appetitive responses at the “food available” level and ultra-processed food (UPF) consumption
Note. * indicates statistical significance (p < .05). Appetitive responses at the “food available” level (PFS Availability subscale score) and delay discounting (AUCDDT) were mean-centered. “Higher” (n = 37) and “lower” (n = 34) delay discounting were determined by respectively subtracting and adding 1 standard deviation (0.35) from and to the adjusted mean (0.00).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Appelhans BM, Woolf K, Pagoto SL, Schneider KL, Whited MC, & Liebman R (2011). Inhibiting food reward: Delay discounting, food reward sensitivity, and palatable food intake in overweight and obese women. Obesity, 19(11), 2175–2182. 10.1038/oby.2011.57 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Arce E, & Santisteban C (2016). Impulsivity: A review. 18(2), 213–220. - PubMed
    1. Baldridge AS, Huffman MD, Taylor F, Xavier D, Bright B, Van Horn LV, Neal B, & Dunford E (2019). The Healthfulness of the US Packaged Food and Beverage Supply: A Cross-Sectional Study. Nutrients, 11(8), 1704. 10.3390/nu11081704 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baraldi LG, Martinez Steele E, Canella DS, & Monteiro CA (2018). Consumption of ultra-processed foods and associated sociodemographic factors in the USA between 2007 and 2012: Evidence from a nationally representative cross-sectional study. BMJ Open, 8(3), e020574. 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-020574 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bechara A (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1458–1463. 10.1038/nn1584 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources