Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Editorial
. 2024 May 3:13:e98274.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.98274.

Memorable first impressions

Affiliations
Editorial

Memorable first impressions

Emilio Salinas et al. Elife. .

Abstract

Our ability to recall details from a remembered image depends on a single mechanism that is engaged from the very moment the image disappears from view.

Keywords: delay encoding; human; iconic memory; neuroscience; population coding; short-term memory; temporal dynamics.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

ES, BS No competing interests declared

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Timeline of events during stimulus presentation and storage.
A visual stimulus (grey box containing pattern) with N items is presented for a period of time (pale blue region). Sensory activity increases to a maximum value during this period, and then decays when the stimulus disappears. For each item, VWM activity also increases towards an effective saturation limit, which is the maximum possible value divided by the number of items presented: here N=2, so the effective saturation limit is half the maximum possible value. When the target item is cued (black arrow; top) at a later time (yellow region), the non-target item(s) are removed from memory (grey trace), and activity associated with the target item (green trace) increases towards the maximum possible value. The level of activity (and hence the accuracy of memory recall) will vary more and more over time due to noise. VWM: visual working memory.

Comment on

  • doi: 10.7554/eLife.91034

References

    1. Baddeley A. Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201. - DOI - PubMed
    1. DePasquale B, Cueva CJ, Rajan K, Escola GS, Abbott LF. full-FORCE: A target-based method for training recurrent networks. PLOS ONE. 2018;13:e0191527. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191527. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. D’Esposito M, Postle BR. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66:115–142. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ma WJ, Husain M, Bays PM. Changing concepts of working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:347–356. doi: 10.1038/nn.3655. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Pasternak T, Greenlee MW. Working memory in primate sensory systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:97–107. doi: 10.1038/nrn1603. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types