Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Jun 6;22(6):e3002644.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002644. eCollection 2024 Jun.

Cumulative route improvements spontaneously emerge in artificial navigators even in the absence of sophisticated communication or thought

Affiliations

Cumulative route improvements spontaneously emerge in artificial navigators even in the absence of sophisticated communication or thought

Edwin S Dalmaijer. PLoS Biol. .

Abstract

Homing pigeons (Columba livia) navigate by solar and magnetic compass, and fly home in idiosyncratic but stable routes when repeatedly released from the same location. However, when experienced pigeons fly alongside naive counterparts, their path is altered. Over several generations of turnover (pairs in which the most experienced individual is replaced with a naive one), pigeons show cumulative improvements in efficiency. Here, I show that such cumulative route improvements can occur in a much simpler system by using agent-based simulation. Artificial agents are in silico entities that navigate with a minimal cognitive architecture of goal-direction (they know roughly where the goal is), social proximity (they seek proximity to others and align headings), route memory (they recall landmarks with increasing precision), and continuity (they avoid erratic turns). Agents' behaviour qualitatively matched that of pigeons, and quantitatively fitted to pigeon data. My results indicate that naive agents benefitted from being paired with experienced agents by following their previously established route. Importantly, experienced agents also benefitted from being paired with naive agents due to regression to the goal: naive agents were more likely to err towards the goal from the perspective of experienced agents' memorised paths. This subtly biased pairs in the goal direction, resulting in intergenerational improvements of route efficiency. No cumulative improvements were evident in control studies in which agents' goal-direction, social proximity, or memory were lesioned. These 3 factors are thus necessary and sufficient for cumulative route improvements to emerge, even in the absence of sophisticated communication or thought.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. The top panel shows paths from artificial agents (introduced here) and from pigeon data published by others [20].
Each line represents the final flight in a generation. The first generation comprises a single individual; a naive individual was added in the second; and in all later generations the most experienced was replaced with a naive individual. Solid lines show lone or experienced individuals, dotted lines show naive ones. The bottom panel shows how agents navigated by sampling from a weighted mixture of Von Mises distributions. These were centred on bearings towards the goal (green), other agents (blue), landmarks along a memorised route (purple), and the previous heading (yellow). Bottom left shows these distributions in a radial plot, with arrows indicating component centres and weights. Bottom right shows the distributions and their weighted sum (black). Artwork used in this figure exists in the public domain or was released under a CC0 license and can be found on Wikimedia Commons (https://commons.wikimedia.org/) under file names F1_chequered_flag.svg, Google_Maps_pin.svg, RockDove.jpg, and Black_rock_pigeon.jpg.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Progression of route efficiency as a function of flight number.
The top panel shows results for the optimum for final efficiency, the middle for the optimum for intergenerational improvement, and the bottom panel for pigeon data published by others [20]. Lines show mean values over independent runs, with 95% confidence intervals as shaded areas. In the experimental condition, a naive agent replaced an experienced one in each generation; in the solo condition, a single agent made all journeys with no generational turnover; and in the pair condition, 2 agents journeyed together without turnover. Parameters for the navigation model were the same between each of the 3 conditions, and weights are listed above each panel.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Each panel shows the difference in route efficiency between naive agents in the experimental condition (generational turnover) and the first 12 journeys from agents in the pair control condition (without generational turnover).
Positive differences indicate that naive agents had better route efficiency compared to control. Each panel represents a combination of wgoal and wsocial parameters, while darker lines indicate higher levels of wmemory. Lines represent averages across 50 independent runs and their shaded areas the 95% confidence interval.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Each panel shows the distribution of relative bearings towards the naive agent from the perspective of the experienced agent in generations 2–5 of the experimental condition.
Positive values on the x-axis indicate bearings towards the goal, and negative values bearings away from the goal. Distributions are generally right-heavy, indicating a bias of naive individuals to be positioned in the general direction of the goal. This tendency increases as a function of wsocial and to a lesser extent as a function of wgoal.

Similar articles

References

    1. Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare. Proc Br Acad. 1996;88:77–93.
    1. Tomasello M. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA, USA: Harvard University Press; 1999.
    1. Tomasello M, Kruger AC, Ratner HH. Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci. 1993. Sep;16(3):495–511.
    1. Derex M. Human cumulative culture and the exploitation of natural phenomena. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2022. Jan 31;377(1843):20200311. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0311 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M. Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2009. Aug 27;364(1528):2405–15. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0052 - DOI - PMC - PubMed